OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America

Journal of the Optical Society of America

  • Vol. 65, Iss. 10 — Oct. 1, 1975
  • pp: 1121–1128

Night myopia and the intermediate dark focus of accommodation

H. W. Leibowitz and D. A. Owens  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA, Vol. 65, Issue 10, pp. 1121-1128 (1975)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSA.65.001121


View Full Text Article

Acrobat PDF (1014 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

The phenomenon of night myopia, the tendency to overaccommodate for distant objects as luminance is decreased, results from the passive return of accommodation to an individually determined intermediate resting or dark focus. More generally, accommodation is viewed as a compromise between the subject's individual resting focus and the accommodative stimulus. Under optimum viewing conditions, accommodation tends to correspond to the distance of the stimulus, but is biased progressively toward the dark focus as the adequady of the accommodative stimulus is degraded by decreased luminance. Control experiments suggest that optical aberrations are not major factors that contribute to this effect.

Citation
H. W. Leibowitz and D. A. Owens, "Night myopia and the intermediate dark focus of accommodation," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 65, 1121-1128 (1975)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josa/abstract.cfm?URI=josa-65-10-1121


Sort:  Author  |  Journal  |  Reset

References

  1. J. R. Levine, R. Soc. Lond., Notes and Reports 20, 100 (1965).
  2. M. Koomen, R. Scolnik, and R. Tousey. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 41, 80 (1951).
  3. H. A. Knoll, Am. J. Optom. 29, 69 (1952).
  4. H. A. W. Schober, Optik 11, 282 (1954).
  5. J. Mellerio, Vision Res. 6, 217 (1966).
  6. I. M. Borish, Clinical Refraction (Professional Press, New York, 1970).
  7. H. Schober, Das Sehen, Vol. II, 68 (VEB Fachbuchverlag, Leipzig, 1964), has pointed out that the defocused image that results from night myopia may serve to increase light sensitivity under low-luminance conditions, in a manner analogous to the tapetum lucidem of lower forms.
  8. F. M. Toates, Physiol. Rev. 52, 828 (1972).
  9. G. Wald and D. R. Griffin, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 37, 321 (1947).
  10. J. M. Otero, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 41, 942 (1951).
  11. M. Luckiesh and F. K. Moss, J. Expt. Psychol. 26, 352 (1940).
  12. F. W. Campbell and J. A. F. Primrose. Trans. Opt Soc. 73, 353 (1953).
  13. M. Alpern and H. David, Ind. Med. Sur. 27, 551 (1958).
  14. E. F. Fincham, Vision Res. 1, 425 (1962).
  15. G. Westheimer, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 47, 714 (1957).
  16. G. Westheimer and S. M. Blair, Vision Res. 13, 1035 (1973).
  17. M. W. Morgan, Am. J. Optom. 23, 99 (1946); 34, 347 (1957); Brit. J. Physiol. Opt. 15, 154 (1958).
  18. R. T. Hennessy and H. W. Leibowitz, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 60, 1700 (1970); Beh. Res. Meth. Inst. 4, 237 (1972); H. W. Leibowitz and R. T. Hennessy, Am. Psychologist 30, 349 (1975).
  19. A recent article by W. N. Charman, Am. J. Optom. Physiol. Opt. 51, 832 (1974), indicates that the plane of no apparent motion, or stationarity, for the laser optometers used in the present experiments actually falls at varying positions between the axis and surface of the drum. Thus, our assuming the axis as the reference point results in a small error of underestimation of the accommodative response, which increases with increased accommodation.
  20. H. A. Knoll, Am. J. Optom. 43, 415 (1966).
  21. W. R. Baldwin and W. B. Stover, Am. J. Optom. 45, 143 (1968).
  22. F. Ingelstam and S. I. Ragnarsson, Vision Res. 12, 411 (1972).
  23. Institute for Perception TNO, Soesterberg, Netherlands.
  24. Since all subjects demonstrated at least 20/20 near and far acuity, large refractive errors are considered unlikely. Any small remaining refractive errors would not have affected either the correlation values nor the nature of the present results.
  25. The authors are indebted to Dr. Lewis O. Harvey, Jr. and to John T. Fisher, M.D. and Channing Nicholas, M. D. for their advice and assistance.
  26. The combined effect of spherical and chromatic aberrations has been estimated to be only 0.5–0.75 D, which is considerably less than the magnitude of night myopia shown by the majority of the subjects in the present study. Spherical aberration, as well as other effects associated with large pupils, should be exaggerated by pupil dilation; this was not found in Experiment III. The possible contribution of these factors, in addition to those of chromatic aberration and the Purkinje shift, seem to be minimized by the fact that all luminance levels in the present study were photopic.
  27. J. Palacios, Investigacion y Progresso 14, 267 (1943).
  28. C. A. Johnson, Ph.D. dissertation (Pennsylvania State University, 1974) University Microfilms, Ann Arbor, Mich. (1974).
  29. R. T. Hennessy, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 65, 1114 (1975).
  30. H. W. Leibowitz, R. T. Hennessy, and D. A. Owens, Psychologia 18, Sept. (1975).
  31. H. W. Leibowitz and D. A. Owens, Science 189, 646 (1975).

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.

« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited