OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America

Journal of the Optical Society of America

  • Vol. 45, Iss. 7 — Jul. 1, 1955
  • pp: 546–552

Some Quantitative Aspects of an Opponent-Colors Theory. I. Chromatic Responses and Spectral Saturation

DOROTHEA JAMESON and LEO M. HURVICH  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA, Vol. 45, Issue 7, pp. 546-552 (1955)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSA.45.000546


View Full Text Article

Acrobat PDF (939 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

Saturation discrimination is assumed to be dependent on the ratio of chromatic to achromatic components in the sensory response to a given wavelength. The usual methods of measurement do not, however, permit independent control of the chromatic and achromatic variables. On the basis of an opponent-colors theory of vision, a method is described for measuring directly and separately the spectral distributions of the chromatic components. A series of experiments is reported in which this method was used to obtain measures of the paired chromatic responses associated with the four primary spectral hues. Results are reported for two observers, 1° foveal test field, for an equal brightness spectrum (10 mL), and a neutral state of adaptation (10 mL). The measured chromatic responses, together with achromatic (luminosity) functions measured earlier for the same observers, are used to predict the form of the spectral saturation discrimination function.

Citation
DOROTHEA JAMESON and LEO M. HURVICH, "Some Quantitative Aspects of an Opponent-Colors Theory. I. Chromatic Responses and Spectral Saturation," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 45, 546-552 (1955)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josa/abstract.cfm?URI=josa-45-7-546


Sort:  Author  |  Journal  |  Reset

References

  1. L. T. Troland, The Principles of Psychophysiology (D. Van Nostrand Company, Inc., New York, 1930), Vol. II, p. 147.
  2. J. H. Parsons, An Introduction to the Study of Colour Vision (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, England, 1924), pp. 31–32.
  3. D. M. Purdy, "Chroma as a function of retinal illumination," dissertation, Harvard University, 1–190 (1929).
  4. I. G. Priest and F. G. Brickwedde, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 28, 133 (1938).
  5. Martin, Warburton, and Morgan, Med. Research Council (Brit.), Spec. Rept. Ser. No. 188, (1933).
  6. W. D. Wright and F. H. G. Pitt, Proc. Phys. Soc. (London) 49, 329 (1937).
  7. J. H. Nelson, Proc. Phys. Soc. (London) 49, 332 (1937).
  8. W. F. Grether, J. Exptl. Psychol. 28, 419 (1941).
  9. A. Chapanis, J. Exptl. Psychol. 34, 24 (1944).
  10. L. A. Jones and E. M. Lowry, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 13, 25 (1926).
  11. L. T. Troland, Trans. Illum. Eng. Soc. (N. Y.) 11, 947 (1916).
  12. R. H. Sinden, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 7, 1123 (1923).
  13. D. M. Purdy, Brit. J. Psychol. 21, 283 (1931).
  14. W. D. Wright and F. H. G. Pitt, Proc. Phys. Soc. (London) 47, 205 (1935).
  15. W. D. Wright, Researches on Normal and Defective Colour Vision (C. V. Mosby Company, St. Louis, 1947), p. 165.
  16. D. B. Judd, Natl. Bur. Standards (U. S.) 4, 515 (1930).
  17. S. Hecht, A Handbook of General Experimental Psychology (edited by C. Murchison) (Clark University Press, Massachusetts, 1934), pp. 704–828.
  18. D. B. Judd, Natl. Bur. Standards (U. S.) 42, 1 (1949).
  19. Reference 17, p. 792.
  20. E. Hering, Sitzber. Akad. Wiss. Wien Math.-naturw. Klasse. 70, 169 (1875).
  21. A. Brückner, Z. Sinnesphysiol. 58, 322 (1927).
  22. Two other experiments of a similar nature have been reported [L. Goldytsch, Z. Biol. 67, 35 (1917); E. Brücke and N. Inouye, Pflüger's Arch. ges. Physiol. 141, 573 (1911)]. In one, only a single chromatic response function (yellow) was measured, in the other, chromatic valence curves were determined for a color-blind observer.
  23. L. M. Hurvich and D. Jameson, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 43, 485 (1953).
  24. D. Jameson and L. M. Hurvich, J. Exptl. Psychol. 41, 455 (1951).
  25. The phrase "chromatic valences" as used here is not to be confused with Hering's "optical valences" which refer to stimulus properties or capacities relative to a single, fixed state of adaptation (reference 26). Nor should the phrase "chromatic response" be confused with the so-called "fundamental response curves" or receptor sensitivity curves.
  26. E. Hering, Pflüger's Arch. ges. Physiol. 57, 308 (1894).
  27. E. Hering, Grundzüge der Lehre vom Lichtsinn (Verlag Julius Springer, Berlin, 1920), p. 288 ff.
  28. Complete numerical data with a sigma values are available from the authors upon request.
  29. A. v. Tschermak-Seysenegg, Naturwiss. 18, 589 (1930).
  30. E. Hering, Lotos, Jahrb. Naturwiss. N.F. 1, 81 (1880).
  31. Reference 15, pp. 165–166.
  32. L. C. Thomson, Ophthal. Lit. 6, 14 (1952).
  33. H. v. Helmholtz, Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik (L. Voss, Hamburg and Leipzig, 1896), second edition, p. 456.
  34. Reference 30, p. 77.

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.

« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited