OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America

Journal of the Optical Society of America

  • Vol. 52, Iss. 5 — May. 1, 1962
  • pp: 565–568

Geometry of a Visual Illusion

MAURICE M. TAYLOR  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA, Vol. 52, Issue 5, pp. 565-568 (1962)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSA.52.000565


View Full Text Article

Acrobat PDF (617 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

A new visual illusion is predicted from the assumption that the perceived distance along any path depends on the discriminability for position along the path. A disk is placed between two dots, so that the straight path between the dots is nearly tangent to the disk. It is predicted that, for the perceived straight path between the dots to be tangent to the disk, the disk must overlap the physically straight path between the dots by an amount proportional to its radius. Furthermore, certain patternings of the disk are predicted to reduce the amount of illusion. All predictions are confirmed in detail. The results are compared with those obtained in an experiment on the filled-space illusion.

Citation
MAURICE M. TAYLOR, "Geometry of a Visual Illusion," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 52, 565-568 (1962)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josa/abstract.cfm?URI=josa-52-5-565


Sort:  Author  |  Journal  |  Reset

References

  1. Anonymous, "Popular Scientific Recreations, a storehouse of instruction and amusement: in which the Marvels of Natural Philosphy, Chemistry, Geology, Astronomy, etc., are explained and illustrated, mainly by means of pleasing experiments and attractive pastimes." Translated and enlarged from Gaston Tissandier, Les Recreations Scientifiques, New and Enlarged Edition [Ward, Lock, Bowden and co., London, 1882 (?)], p. 113. The statement does not appear in the original.
  2. W. R. Garner, J. Exptl. Psychol. 43, 232–238 (1952); J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 30, 1005–1012 (1958); M. M. Taylor, Can. J. Psychol. (to be published).
  3. S. S. Stevens and E. H. Galanter, J. Exptl. Psychol. 54, 377–411 (1957); S. S. Stevens, Am. J. Psychol. 71, 633–646 (1958).
  4. W. P. Tanner, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 30, 922–928 (1958); see also D. M. Green, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 32, 1189–1202 (1960).
  5. W. R. Garner, reference 2 (1952).
  6. M. M. Taylor, Perceptual and Motor Skills 12, 203–30 (1961).
  7. This expansion may be obtained from the basic equation by using M instead of M2, so that the term (∂D/∂M2)(dM2/dA) is replaced by (∂D/∂M)(dM/dA)=kA/L(∂D/∂M). ∂D/∂M is always evaluated for M = L, so that it is a constant, and this constant will probably not be far from unity, compared with the range of uncertainty in the value of k. For purposes of calculation in this section, it is assumed that ∂D/∂M = 1.
  8. H. G. Spiegel, Psychol. Forsch. 21, 327–83 (1937). A summary in English of his main result is given by Taylor, reference 6.

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.

« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited