OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America

Journal of the Optical Society of America

  • Vol. 59, Iss. 5 — May. 1, 1969
  • pp: 617–623

Saccadic Suppression

WHITMAN RICHARDS  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA, Vol. 59, Issue 5, pp. 617-623 (1969)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSA.59.000617


View Full Text Article

Acrobat PDF (799 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

A shift of the peak of the Stiles-Crawford effect suggests that saccades shear the retina. This action appears to lead to an increase of the retinal activity of a real-light background. Thus, thresholds following a saccade are raised the most for test wavelengths which are most similar to the adapting-field wavelength. If the adapting field is eliminated, saccadic suppression is reduced. Saccades also affect the customary rises of thresholds found near the onset and extinction of the adapting field. This effect is as if the retinal feedback loop underlying adaptation is disrupted by the saccade.

Citation
WHITMAN RICHARDS, "Saccadic Suppression," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 59, 617-623 (1969)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josa/abstract.cfm?URI=josa-59-5-617


Sort:  Author  |  Journal  |  Reset

References

  1. P. L. Latour, Vision Res. 2, 261 (1962).
  2. B. L. Zuber and L. Stark, Exptl. Neurol. 16, 65 (1966).
  3. F. C. Volkmann, A. M. L. Schick, and L. A. Riggs, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 58, 562 (1968).
  4. P. L. Latour, "Cortical Control of Eye Movement," thesis, Institute for Perception RVO-TNO, Soesterberg, The Netherlands (1966).
  5. H.-L. Teuber, in Handbook of Physiology, Vol. III, Neurophysiology, J. Field, Ed. (American Physiological Society, Washington, D. C., 1960), p. 1595.
  6. E. B. Holt, Harvard Psychol. Stud. I, 3 (1903).
  7. W. Richards, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 58, 1159 (1968).
  8. Note also the phosphene of quick eye motion reported by B. Nebel, Arch. Ophthalmol. 58, 236 (1957).
  9. L. Stark, G. Vossius, and L. R. Young, IRE Trans. HFE-3, 52 (1962).
  10. B. H. Crawford, Proc. Roy. Soc. (London) B134, 283 (1947).
  11. H. B. Barlow and J. M. B. Sparrock, Science 144, 1309 (1964).
  12. W. A. H. Rushton, Proc. Roy. Soc. (London) B162, 20 (1965).
  13. J. E. Dowling, Science 155, 273 (1967).
  14. R. M. Chapman, Vision Res. 2, 89 (1962); R. A. Cone, J. Gen. Physiol. 47, 1089 (1964).
  15. Note also the characteristic secondary decrease of the threshold near 180 to 200 msec equivalent to 50 or 70 msec prior to the beginning of the return saccade. This decrease has also been reported by others.1–4 We propose that the peak and valley preceding the eye movement reflect neural disinhibition or the Broca-Sulzer phenomenon, whereas those decreases that occur after a single eye movement are due to retinal (or vitreous) oscillations.
  16. H. D. Baker, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 39, 172 (1949); 53, 98 (1963).
  17. R. M. Boynton, Arch. Ophthalmol. 60, 800 (1958).
  18. F. Ratliff, H. K. Hartline, and W. H. Miller, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 53, 110(1963).
  19. G. Westheimer, J. Physiol. (London) 190, 139 (1967).

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.

« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited