OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America

Journal of the Optical Society of America

  • Vol. 63, Iss. 7 — Jul. 1, 1973
  • pp: 884–888

Brightness of isolated colored lights

C. R. Cavonius and R. Hilz  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA, Vol. 63, Issue 7, pp. 884-888 (1973)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSA.63.000884


View Full Text Article

Acrobat PDF (587 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

The brightness of 2° centrally fixated monochromatic lights was measured by three methods: magnitude estimation, in which the observer made numerical estimates of brightness; delayed matching, in which he adjusted the luminance of an achromatic field until it matched the remembered brightness of the monochromatic field; and conventional heterochromatic photometry. The photometry data resemble the CIE Vλ function, but both the estimation and delayed-matching procedures result in substantially higher sensitivities to short-wavelength stimuli. Part of this excess sensitivity is due to scotopic intrusion and can be eliminated by light adaptation. The remaining sensitivity to short-wavelength stimuli resembles the sensitivity of peripheral cones.

Citation
C. R. Cavonius and R. Hilz, "Brightness of isolated colored lights," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 63, 884-888 (1973)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josa/abstract.cfm?URI=josa-63-7-884


Sort:  Author  |  Journal  |  Reset

References

  1. L. E. Marks, Percept. Psychophys. 9, 26 (1971).
  2. C. A. Padgham, Vision Res. 11, 577 (1971).
  3. D. Yager, Vision Res. 10, 521 (1970).
  4. S. S. Stevens, in Handbook of Sensory Physiology, edited by W. R. Loewenstein (Springer, Berlin, 1971), Vol. 1, Ch. 7.
  5. S. M. Newhall, R. W. Burnham, and J. R. Clark, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 47, 43 (1957).
  6. J. W. T. Walsh, Photometry (London, Constable, 1958), p. 203.
  7. L. W. Marks and J. C. Stevens, Percept. Psychophys. 1, 17 (1966); Percept. Psychophys. 4, 315 (1968).
  8. The data shown in the figures may be obtained in tabular form from the authors, or from the National Auxiliary Publications Service, c/o Microfiche Publications, 305 East 46 Street, New York, N.Y. 10017.
  9. G. Ekman, Studium Generale 16, 54 (1963).
  10. Essentially the same mean sensitivity data can be obtained from the group-brightness-estimation plots (Fig. 1). However, the low-luminance ends of these functions are distorted because some observers' slopes change in this region whereas others do not. To be certain that our criterion fell on thc main part of each observer's estimation function, we first obtained each individual's spectral sensitivity function and then took the mean of these individual functions.
  11. Y. Hsia and C. H. Graham, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 38, 80 (1952).
  12. H. G. Sperling and Y. Hsia, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 47, 707 (1957).
  13. B. C. Wilson, Ph.D. thesis, New York University (1964). Cited by D. H. Krantz, in Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol. VII/4, edited by D. Jameson and L. M. Hurvich (Springer, Berlin, 1972), p. 678.
  14. S. L. Guth, in Color Metrics, edited by J. J. Vos, L. F. C. Friele, and P. L. Walraven (AIC/Holland, Soesterberg, 1972).

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.

« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited