OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America

Journal of the Optical Society of America

  • Vol. 69, Iss. 9 — Sep. 1, 1979
  • pp: 1259–1265

Large-field color naming of dichromats with rods bleached

Allen L. Nagy and Robert M. Boynton  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA, Vol. 69, Issue 9, pp. 1259-1265 (1979)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSA.69.001259


View Full Text Article

Acrobat PDF (1050 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

A color-naming method was used to examine the large-field red/green discriminations of dlchromats screened with standard tests. The stimulus was a 12°. annular field with the central 4°. removed, flashed for 300 ms. Four wavelengths were equated in brightness for each observer at two retinal illuminance levels, approximately 10 and 100 trolands. The stimuli were then presented in random order and the observer was asked to name each, using one of four color terms. The entire experiment was done with the observers dark-adapted and also with the rods bleached. For all four deuteranopes and two of four protanopes, color names were very significantly related to both illuminance and wavelength in both adaptation conditions. The relationship between name and wavelength was similar to that of a normal trichromat, but the performance of a dichromat was very poor by comparison. Performance was generally somewhat better in the dark than with the rods bleached. However, the result in the bleached condition is consistent with recent evidence that at least some observers who are classified as dichromats with standard small-field screening procedures actually have a weak residual third cone mechanism.

© 1979 Optical Society of America

Citation
Allen L. Nagy and Robert M. Boynton, "Large-field color naming of dichromats with rods bleached," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 69, 1259-1265 (1979)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josa/abstract.cfm?URI=josa-69-9-1259


Sort:  Author  |  Year  |  Journal  |  Reset

References

  1. W. A. Nagel, "Dichromatische Fovea, trichromatische Peripherie," Z. Psychol. Physiol. Sinnes. 39, 93–101 (1905).
  2. W. A. Nagel, "Neue Effrahrungen uber das Farbensehen der dichromaten auf grossen felde," Z. Sinnesphysiol. 41, 319–337 (1907).
  3. W. A. Nagel, "Farbenstimmung beim Dichromaten," Z. Sinnesphysiol. 44, 5–17 (1910).
  4. W. Jaeger and K. Kroker, "Uber das Verhalten der Protanopen und Deuteranopen bei grossen Reizflachen," Klin MBL. Augenhelk. 121, 445–449 (1952).
  5. V. C. Smith and J. Pokorny, "Large-field trichromacy in protanopes and deuteranopes," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 67, 213–220 (1977).
  6. H. M. O. Scheibner and R. M. Boynton, "Residual red-green discrimination in dichromats," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 58, 1151–1158 (1968).
  7. F. Frome, T. Piantanida, and D. H. Kelly, "Flicker thresholds reveal forbidden cones in dichromats," Supplement to Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. 17, 197 (1978).
  8. T. P. Piantanida, F. Frome, and D. H. Kelly, "Spectral sensitivity of the forbidden cones in dichromats," Supplement to Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. 17, 197–198 (1978).
  9. M. Alpern and T. Wake, "Cone pigments in human deutan colour vision defects," J. Physiol. 266, 595–612 (1977).
  10. Subsequent to the preparation of this manuscript one protanope and one deuteranope were run using a "Ganzfield" bleach, obtained with a slide projector beam illuminating a half of a ping-pong ball mounted in the bottom of a coffee cup [See T. R. Corwin and M. A. Green, "The Broca-Sulzer effect in a ganzfield," Vis. Res. 18, 1675–1678, (1978)]. The observer placed his eye to the ping-pong ball to obtain a reasonably homogeneous bleach of the entire retina. Color-naming data were obtained following the same procedures used in the bleaching experiments described above. Both observers performed about as well as they did in experiments with 20° bleaches. This result indicates that the results described in the body of the paper cannot be attributed to stray light from the test flashes falling on unbleached rods.
  11. C. E. Shannon, "A mathematical theory of communication," Bell Syst. Tech. J. 27, 379–423 (1948).
  12. p. Attneave, Applications of Information Theory to Psychology (Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, New York, 1959).
  13. W. L. Hays, Statistics (Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, New York, 1963).
  14. G. A. Miller and W. G. Madow, "On the maximum likelihood estimates of the Shannon-Weiner measure of information," Air Force Cambridge Research Center: Tech. Rpt. 54–75 (August, 1954) (unpublished).
  15. W. A. H. Rushton, "Rhodopsin measurement and dark adaptation in a subject deficient in cone vision," J. Physiol. 156, 193–205 (1961).
  16. M. Alpern, W. A. H. Rushton, and S. Torii, "The attenuation of rod signals by bleaching," J. Physiol. 207, 449–461 (1970).
  17. C. J. Gosline, D. I. A. MacLeod, and W. A. H. Rushton, "The dark adaptation curve of rods measured by their after-image," J. Physiol. 259, 491–499 (1976).
  18. M. M. Hayhoe, D. I. A. MacLeod, and T. A. Bruch, "Rod-cone independence in dark adaptation," Vis. Res. 16, 591–600 (1976).
  19. B. R. Wooten and G. Wald, "Colour-vision mechanisms in peripheral retinas of normal and dichromatic observers," J. Gen. Physiol. 61, 125–145 (1973).
  20. J. Gordon and I. Abramov, "Color vision in the peripheral retina. II. Hue and saturation," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 67, 202–206 (1977).
  21. W. A. H. Rushton, "Discussion on human color vision," edited by A. V. S. DeReuck and J. Knight, in Ciba Foundation Symposium on Color Vision (Little, Brown, Boston, 1965). pp. 246–247.
  22. M. Alpern, "Distal mechanisms of vertebrate color vision," Ann. Rev. Physiol. 30, 279–318 (1968).
  23. J. J. Vos and P. L. Walraven, "On the derivation of the foveal receptor primaries," Vision Res. 11, 799–818 (1971).
  24. V. C. Smith and J. Pokorny, "Spectral sensitivity of color-blind observers and the cone photopigments," Vision Res. 12, 2059–2071 (1972).

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.

« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited