OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America

Journal of the Optical Society of America

  • Vol. 70, Iss. 11 — Nov. 1, 1980
  • pp: 1283–1289

Motion and vision. III. Stabilized pattern adaptation

D.H. Kelly and Christina A. Burbeck  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA, Vol. 70, Issue 11, pp. 1283-1289 (1980)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSA.70.001283


View Full Text Article

Acrobat PDF (956 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

It has been suggested that local variations of retinal sensitivity may be responsible for elevating the threshold in pattern-adaptation experiments of the Blakemore-Campbell type. Subjects are unable to scan high-contrast gratings uniformly enough to eliminate this possibility. To control this effect, we performed grating-adaptation experiments under stabilized-image conditions, while both adapting and test targets were moved at retinal velocities determined by the experimenter. By means of an afterimage technique, we also measured the strength of the retinal sensitivity mask that forms under these conditions. Varying the spatial frequency and velocity of the adapting stimulus, we inferred the spatial and temporal properties of the principal mechanism that contributes to the afterimage. We found that the Blakemore-Campbell effect persists at adapting velocities that are fast enough to rule out local variations of retinal sensitivity. More surprisingly, even the clearly visible afterimages that occur at a retinal velocity of 0.1 deg/s seem to have no effect on pattern adaptation. (Sensitivity masking can raise the adapted threshold, but only at adapting velocities slower than normal eye movements.) By manipulating the image velocity, we were able to shift the spatial frequencies of some threshold-elevation curves, but these shifts were not great enough to suggest that velocity tuning plays important role in pattern adaptation.

© 1980 Optical Society of America

Citation
D.H. Kelly and Christina A. Burbeck, "Motion and vision. III. Stabilized pattern adaptation," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 70, 1283-1289 (1980)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josa/abstract.cfm?URI=josa-70-11-1283


Sort:  Author  |  Year  |  Journal  |  Reset

References

  1. D. H. Kelly, "Motion and vision. I. Stabilized images of stationary gratings," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 69, 1266–1274 (1979).
  2. D. H. Kelly, "Visual contrast sensitivity," Opt. Acta 24, 107–129 (1977).
  3. D. H. Kelly, "Motion and vision. II. Stabilized spatio-temporal threshold surface," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 69, 1340–1349 (1979).
  4. C. Blakemore and F. W. Campbell, "On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images," J. Physiol. (London) 203, 237–260 (1969).
  5. R. M. Jones and U. Tulunay-Keesey, "Local retinal adaptation and spatial frequency channels," Vision Res. 15, 1239–1244 (1975).
  6. R. M. Jones and U. Tulunay-Keesey, "Phase selectivity of spatial frequency channels," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 70, 66–70 (1980).
  7. N. Graham, "Spatial frequency channels in the human visual system: Effects of luminance and pattern drift rate," Vision Res. 12, 53–68 (1972).
  8. D. J. Tolhurst, "Separate channels for the analysis of the shape and the movements of a moving visual stimulus," J. Physiol. (London) 231, 385–402 (1973).
  9. L. E. Arend and A. A. Skavenski, "Free scanning of gratings produces patterned retinal exposure," Vision Res. 19, 1413–1419 (1979).
  10. Conversely, if the duration of a transient stimulus is short enough to "freeze" any natural eye movement, then stabilizing the retinal image obviously can have no effect. In the intermediate range of longer transients, the effect of stabilization seems to be variable and difficult to interpret. By comparison, steady-state stimulation is a much more revealing technique for exploring stabilized-image effects.
  11. D. H. Kelly and R. E. Savoie, "A study of sine-wave contrast sensitivity by two psychophysical methods," Percept. Psychophys. 14, 313–318 (1973).
  12. Samples of our eye-movement data were originally included in this paper, but were subsequently deleted at the suggestion of a referee.
  13. D. H. Fender (personal communication).
  14. J. J. Koenderink, "Contrast enhancement and the negative after-image," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 62, 685–689 (1972).
  15. D. H. Kelly, "Theory of flicker and transient responses. I. Uniform fields," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 61, 537–546 (1971).
  16. D. H. Kelly, "Adaptation effects on spatio-temporal sine-wave thresholds," Vision Res. 12, 89–101 (1972).
  17. The bandwidths shown in Fig. 5 may appear broader than those of Refs. 4, 5, and others, but this is due to a different way of plotting the data. Blakemore and Campbell subtracted 1 from their threshold elevation ratios and plotted the result on a logarithmic scale, whereas we plot the log of the ratio directly.
  18. This effect has been studied in detail by Karen DeValois, "Spatial frequency adaptation can enhance contrast sensitivity," Vision Res. 17, 1057–1065 (1977).
  19. See Ref. 3, Fig. 11.
  20. F. L. Van Nes, J. J. Koenderink, H. Nas, and M. A. Bouman, "Spatiotemporal modulation transfer in the human eye," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 57, 1082–1088 (1967).
  21. D. H. Kelly, "Frequency doubling in visual responses," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 56, 1628–1633 (1966).
  22. V. Virsu and P. Laurinen, "Long-lasting afterimages caused by neural adaptation," Vision Res. 17, 853–860 (1977).

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.

Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited