OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

| OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION

  • Vol. 15, Iss. 1 — Jan. 1, 1998
  • pp: 1–15

Spatial-frequency tuning of sustained nonoriented units of the Red–Green channel

Ram L. Pandey Vimal  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA A, Vol. 15, Issue 1, pp. 1-15 (1998)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.15.000001


View Full Text Article

Enhanced HTML    Acrobat PDF (347 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

The existence of nonoriented cells but not of sustained orthogonal masking for achromatic stimuli led to an investigation of the spatial-frequency (SF) tuning of sustained nonoriented color units. For this purpose the Red–Green channel was isolated by the minimum flicker and hue cancellation techniques. Chromatic contrast sensitivity functions (CSF’s), threshold elevation (TE) curves, and contrast nonlinearities (TE-versus-mask-contrast curves) were measured with spatially localized vertical color tests and sinusoidal orthogonal color masks by the method of constant stimuli under Gaussian temporal presentation. Results show that (1) color CSF’s are a low-pass function of SF, whereas TE curves are a bandpass function of mask SF, and (2) a minimum of six SF-tuned color mechanisms (one low-pass and five bandpass functions of SF, with peak SF’s of 0.13, 0.5, 2, 4, and 8 cycles per degree and bandwidths of 3.9, 4.4, 2.9, 2.1, 1.1, and 1.2 octaves), similar to oblique-masking color mechanisms, are extracted by the multiple-mechanism model. These data imply that (1) most of the SF tuning of the broadly oriented color units is already present in the circularly symmetric units, and (2) the latter may be an input to the former.

© 1998 Optical Society of America

OCIS Codes
(330.1800) Vision, color, and visual optics : Vision - contrast sensitivity
(330.6100) Vision, color, and visual optics : Spatial discrimination

History
Original Manuscript: March 17, 1997
Revised Manuscript: July 11, 1997
Manuscript Accepted: May 22, 1997
Published: January 1, 1998

Citation
Ram L. Pandey Vimal, "Spatial-frequency tuning of sustained nonoriented units of the Red–Green channel," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 15, 1-15 (1998)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josaa/abstract.cfm?URI=josaa-15-1-1


Sort:  Author  |  Year  |  Journal  |  Reset  

References

  1. D. H. Hubel, T. N. Wiesel, “Receptive fields and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex,” J. Physiol. (London) 195, 215–243 (1968) and references therein.
  2. L. G. Thorell, R. L. De Valois, D. G. Albrecht, “Spatial mapping of monkey V1 cells with pure color and luminance stimuli,” Vision Res. 24, 751–769 (1984). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. C. R. Michael, “Laminar segregation of color cells in the monkey’s striate cortex,” Vision Res. 25, 415–423 (1985). [CrossRef]
  4. P. Lennie, J. Krauskopf, G. Sclar, “Chromatic mechanisms in striate cortex of macaque,” J. Neurosci. 10, 649–669 (1990). [PubMed]
  5. M. S. Livingstone, D. H. Hubel, “Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth,” J. Neurosci. 7, 3416–3468 (1987) and references therein. The question mark (?) in the text (Section 1), presumably representing the uncertainty in the findings, was originally used by these authors. [PubMed]
  6. R. Pandey, R. L. P. Vimal, “Threshold elevation curves for the Red–Green channel estimated by oblique masking,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. Suppl. 34, 751 (1993); R. L. P. Vimal, R. Pandey, “Spatial frequency tuned mechanisms of the Red–Green channel estimated by oblique masking,” Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 20 (Part 2), 1580 (1994). The MSC analysis for the oblique-masking color mechanisms is available from the author.
  7. H. R. Wilson, D. K. McFarlane, G. C. Phillips, “Spatial frequency tuning of orientation selective units estimated by oblique masking,” Vision Res. 23, 873–882 (1983), and references therein. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. V. P. Ferrera, H. R. Wilson, “Spatial frequency tuning of transient non-oriented units,” Vision Res. 25, 67–72 (1985); G. C. Phillips, H. R. Wilson, “Orientation bandwidths of spatial mechanisms measured by masking,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1, 226–232 (1984). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. G. Wyszecki, W. S. Stiles, Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd ed. (Wiley, New York, 1982), and references therein (for color vision models see pp. 582–689; for other topics see the index on pp. 935–950); R. M. Boynton, Human Color Vision (Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York, 1979) (for an opponent color model, see pp. 211–215; for cone weightings see pp. 154 and 213).
  10. L. M. Hurvich, D. Jameson, “An opponent-process theory of color vision,” Psychol. Rev. 64, 384–404 (1957). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. J. Krauskopf, D. R. Williams, D. W. Heeley, “Cardinal directions of colour space,” Vision Res. 22, 1123–1131 (1982). For cone weightings see J. Krauskopf, H. J. Wu, B. Farell, “Coherence, cardinal directions and higher-order mechanisms,” Vision Res. 36, 1235–1245 (1996). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. A. Chaparro, C. F. Stromeyer, R. E. Kronauer, R. T. Eskew, “Separable Red–Green and luminance detectors for small flashes,” Vision Res. 34, 751–762 (1994), and the relevant references therein; C. F. Stromeyer, R. E. Kronauer, A. Ryu, A. Chaparro, R. T. Eskew, “Contribution of human long-wave and middle-wave cones to motion detection,” J. Physiol. (London) 485, 221–243 (1995); C. R. Cole, T. J. Hine, W. Mcilhagga, “Estimation of linear detection mechanisms for stimuli of medium spatial frequency,” Vision Res. 34, 1267-1278 (1994). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. K. T. Mullen, M. A. Losada, “Evidence for separate pathways for color and luminance detection mechanisms,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 11, 3136–3151 (1994); K. T. Mullen, S. J. Cropper, M. A. Losada, “Absence of linear subthreshold summation between Red–Green and luminance mechanisms over a wide range of spatio-temporal conditions,” Vision Res. 37, 1157–1165 (1997); M. A. Losada, K. T. Mullen, “The spatial tuning of chromatic mechanisms identified by simultaneous masking,” Vision Res. 34, 331–334 (1994). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. E. Switkes, A. Bradley, K. K. De Valois, “Contrast dependence and mechanisms of masking interactions among chromatic and luminance gratings,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 5, 1149–1162 (1988). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. J. G. Daugman, “Two-dimensional spectral analysis of cortical receptive field profile,” Vision Res. 20, 847–856 (1980); M. A. Webster, R. L. De Valois, “Relationship between spatial-frequency and orientation tuning of striate-cortex cells,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2, 1124–1132 (1985); R. L. De Valois, “Spatial processing of luminance and color information,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. 17, 834–835 (1978). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. A. Bradley, E. Switkes, K. K. De Valois, “Orientation and spatial frequency selectivity of adaptation to color and luminance gratings,” Vision Res.28, 841–856 (1988); R. L. De Valois, “Orientation and spatial frequency selectivity: properties and modular organization,” in From Pigments to Perception, A. Valberg, B. B. Lee, eds. (Plenum, New York, 1991), pp. 261–267. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. P. Flanagan, P. Cavanagh, O. E. Favreau, “Independent orientation-selective mechanisms for the cardinal direction of color space,” Vision Res. 30, 769–778 (1990). [CrossRef]
  18. R. L. P. Vimal, “Orientation tuning of the spatial-frequency-tuned mechanisms of the Red–Green channel,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 14, 2622–2632 (1997). At 2 cpd the orientation bandwidth (Fig. 5 of the paper of Vimal in this reference) and also the SF bandwidth of the mechanism of the R–G chromatic channel are similar to those of the achromatic channel (compare the R–G mechanism C4 of Fig. 4 of the present paper with the achromatic mechanisms B and C of Fig. 10 of Wilson et al.7); the CSF for the achromatic channel peaks at approximately 2 cpd for the localized stimulus D6 (see Fig. 9 of Wilson et al.7). Therefore it appears that the spatial (SF and orientation) processing of color and luminance stimuli at 2 cpd is similar. [CrossRef]
  19. K. T. Mullen, “The contrast sensitivity of human colour vision to red/green and blue/yellow chromatic gratings,” J. Physiol. (London) 359, 381–400 (1985).
  20. P. Lennie, M. D’Zmura, “Mechanisms of color vision,” Crit. Rev. Neurobiol. 3, 333–400 (1988). [PubMed]
  21. I. Powell, “Lenses for correcting chromatic aberration of the eye,” Appl. Opt. 20, 4152–4155 (1981). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. R. L. P. Vimal, J. M. Pokorny, V. C. Smith, S. K. Shevell, “Foveal cone thresholds,” Vision Res. 29, 61–78 (1989). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. stepit is a subroutine (the original method) in J. P. Chandler, “stept: direct research optimization solution of least-squares problems,” Indiana University, Chemistry Department, Quantum Chemistry Program Exchange (QCPE) 11, 307 (1976).
  24. R. Bolt, L. Beranek, E. Newman, Curve Fitting Commands: RS/Explore User’s Guide (BBN Research System, Cambridge, Mass., 1986), Book 3, pp. 9-1–9-6.
  25. J. Welkowitz, R. B. Ewen, J. Cohen, Introductory Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences (Academic, New York, 1976); for standard deviation see pp. 51–58, for standard error see pp. 104–133, for chi-square see pp. 242–257, and for statistical tables see pp. 304–307.
  26. H. Akaike, “A new look at statistical mode identification,” IEEE Trans. Autom. Control. 19, 716–723 (1974), and references therein. This is used by R. E. Fredericksen, P. J. Bex, F. A. J. Verstraten, “How big is a Gabor patch, and why should we care?” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 14, 1–12 (1997). [CrossRef]
  27. R. L. P. Vimal, R. Pandey, “Measurement of peak spatial frequency and bandwidth of threshold elevation curves of non-oriented units of the Red–Green channel by horizontal masking,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. Suppl. 34, 782 (1993).
  28. R. L. P. Vimal, R. Pandey, “Interaction between the spatial frequency tuned mechanisms of the Red–Green channel and those of the achromatic channel,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. Suppl. 35, 1370 (1994).
  29. R. L. P. Vimal, “Spatial frequency discriminations: inphase and counter-phase photopic conditions compared,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. 29, 448 (1988); R. L. P. Vimal, R. Pandey, “Spatial frequency discrimination for inphase and counter-phase red–green stimuli: effect of apparent motion,” Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 15 (Part 1), p. 625, No. 249.13 (1989).
  30. R. Pandey, R. L. P. Vimal, “Contrast matching in the Red–Green channel: flattening effect and color-contrast-constancy,” Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 19 (Part 3), 1802 (1993).
  31. V. C. Smith, J. M. Pokorny, S. Starr, “Variability of color mixture data. I. Interobserver variability in the unit coordinates,” Vision Res. 16, 1087–1094 (1976); J. M. Pokorny, V. C. Smith, S. Starr, “Variability of color mixture data. II. The effect of field size,” Vision Res. 16, 1087–1094 (1976). [CrossRef]
  32. M. J. Sankeralli, K. T. Mullen, “Estimation of the L-, M-, and S-cone weights of the postreceptoral detection mechanisms,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 13, 906–915 (1996). [CrossRef]
  33. C. W. Tyler, L. Barghout, L. L. Kontsevich, “Surprises in analyzing the mechanisms underlying threshold elevation functions,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. Suppl. 34, 819 (1993); L. L. Kontsevich, C. W. Tyler, “A simple explanation of how contrast affects performance in detection and discrimination tasks below 3 c/deg,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. Suppl. 34, 782 (1993); J. Yang, Q. Xiaofeng, W. Makous, “Zero frequency masking and a model of contrast sensitivity,” Vision Res. 35, 1965–1978 (1995). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. J. M. Pokorny, University of Chicago, Visual Science Center, 939 East 57th Street, Chicago, Illinois 60637 (personal communication, 1996).
  35. J. J. Kulikowski, “What really limits vision? Conceptual limitations to the assessments of visual functions and the role of interacting channels,” in Limits of Vision, Vol. 5 of Vision and Visual Dysfunctions, J. J. Kulikowski, V. Walsh, I. J. Murray, eds. (Macmillan, London, 1991), pp. 286–329.
  36. J. M. Foley, “Human luminance pattern-vision mechanisms: masking experiments require a new model,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 11, 1710–1719 (1994); A. B. Bonds, “Role of inhibition in the specification of orientation selectivity of cells in the cat striate cortex,” Visual Neurosci. 2, 41–55 (1989); D. J. Heeger, “Normalization of cell responses in cat visual cortex,” Visual Neurosci. 9, 181–197 (1992), and references therein. [CrossRef]
  37. H. R. Wilson, D. J. Gelb, “Modified line-element theory for spatial-frequency and width discrimination,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1, 124–131 (1984). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. W. H. Swanson, H. R. Wilson, S. C. Geise, “Contrast matching data predicted from contrast increment thresholds,” Vision Res. 24, 63–75 (1984); W. H. Swanson, M. A. Georgeson, H. R. Wilson, “Comparison of contrast responses across spatial mechanisms,” Vision Res. 28, 457–459 (1988) and references therein. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. H. R. Wilson, “Responses of spatial mechanisms can explain hyperacuity,” Vision Res. 26, 453–469 (1986). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. H. R. Wilson, “A model for direction selectivity in threshold motion perception,” Biol. Cybern. 51, 213–222 (1985). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. F. W. Campbell, J. G. Robson, “Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings,” J. Physiol. (London) 197, 551–566 (1968); C. Blakemore, F. W. Campbell, “On the existence of neurons in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images,” J. Physiol. (London) 203, 237–260 (1969). For details see H. R. Wilson, “Psychophysical models of spatial vision and hyperacuity,” in Spatial Vision, Vol. 10 of Vision and Visual Dysfunction, D. Regan, ed. (Macmillan, London, 1991), pp. 64–86, and references therein.
  42. H. R. Wilson, “A transducer function for threshold and suprathreshold human vision,” Biol. Cybern. 38, 171–178 (1980). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. M. A. Georgeson, J. M. Georgeson, “Facilitation and masking of briefly presented gratings: time-course and contrast dependence,” Vision Res. 27, 369–379 (1987). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. A. M. Sillito, J. A. Kemp, J. A. Milson, N. Berardi, “A re-evaluation of the mechanism underlying simple cell orientation selectivity,” Brain Res. 194, 517–520 (1980); R. Hess, K. Negishi, O. Creutzfeldt, “The horizontal spread of intracortical inhibition in the visual cortex,” Exp. Brain Res. 22, 415–419 (1975); D. Allison, V. A. Casagrande, A. B. Bonds, “The influence of input from the lower cortical layers on the orientation tuning of upper layer V1 cells in a primate,” Visual Neurosci. 12, 309–320 (1995). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. V. A. Billock, “Consequence of retinal color coding for cortical color decoding,” Science 274, 2118–2119 (1996) [also see the responses by D. M. Dacey, Science 274, 2119 (1996) and R. H. Masland, Science 274, 2119 (1996), and references therein]; D. M. Dacey, B. B. Lee, D. K. Stafford, J. Pokorny, V. C. Smith, “Horizontal cells of the primate retina: cone specificity without spectral opponency,” Science 271, 656 (1996); R. H. Masland, “Unscrambling color vision,” Science 271, 616 (1996); C. R. Ingling, E. Martinez-Uriegas, “The spatiotemporal properties of the r–g x-cell channel,” Vision Res. 25, 33–38 (1985). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. M. Gur, V. Akri, “Isoluminant stimuli may not expose the full contribution of color to visual functioning: spatial contrast sensitivity measurements indicate interaction between color and luminance processing,” Vision Res. 32, 1253–1262 (1992). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. M. A. Webster, J. D. Mollon, “The influence of contrast adaptation on color appearance,” Vision Res. 34, 1993–2020 (1994). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. J. J. Atick, Z. Li, A. N. Redlich, “What does post-adaptation color appearance reveal about cortical color representation?,” Vision Res. 33, 123–129 (1993), and references therein. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. F. Crick, C. Koch, “Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex?” Nature (London) 375, 121–123 (1995); M. Gur, M. Snodderly, “A dissociation between brain activity and perception: chromatically opponent cortical neurons signal chromatic flicker that is not perceived,” Vision Res. 37, 377–382 (1997). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. S. M. Zeki, “The distribution of wavelength and orientation selectivity in different areas of monkey visual cortex,” Proc. R. Soc. London, Ser. B 207, 239–248 (1983). [CrossRef]
  51. D. Y. Ts’o, C. D. Gilbert, “The organization of chromatic and spatial interaction in the primate striate cortex,” J. Neurosci. 8, 1712–1727 (1988).
  52. P. H. Schiller, N. K. Logothetis, E. R. Charles, “Functions of the colour-opponent and broad-band channels of the visual system,” Nature (London) 343, 68–70 (1990). [CrossRef]
  53. M. S. Silverman, D. H. Grosof, R. L. De Valois, S. D. Elfar, “Spatial-frequency organization in primate striate cortex,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 86, 711–715 (1989); D. P. Edwards, K. P. Purpura, E. Kaplan, “Contrast sensitivity and spatial frequency response of primate cortical neurons in and around the cytochrome oxidase blobs,” Vision Res. 35, 1501–1523 (1995). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. R. T. Born, R. B. H. Tootell, “Spatial frequency tuning of single units in macaque supragranular striate cortex,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 88, 7066–7070 (1991). [CrossRef] [PubMed]

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.

Figures

Fig. 1 Fig. 2 Fig. 3
 
Fig. 4 Fig. 5
 

Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited