OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

| OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION

  • Editor: Franco Gori
  • Vol. 29, Iss. 2 — Feb. 1, 2012
  • pp: A133–A143

Estimating illuminant color based on luminance balance of surfaces

Keiji Uchikawa, Kazuho Fukuda, Yusuke Kitazawa, and Donald I. A. MacLeod  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA A, Vol. 29, Issue 2, pp. A133-A143 (2012)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.29.00A133


View Full Text Article

Acrobat PDF (1362 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

To accomplish color constancy the illuminant color needs to be discounted from the light reflected from surfaces. Some strategies for discounting the illuminant color use statistics of luminance and chromaticity distribution in natural scenes. In this study we showed whether color constancy exploits the potential cue that was provided by the luminance balance of differently colored surfaces. In our experiments we used six colors: bright and dim red, green, and blue, as surrounding stimulus colors. In most cases, bright colors were set to be optimal colors. They were arranged among 60 hexagonal elements in close-packed structure. The center element served as the test stimulus. The observer adjusted the chromaticity of the test stimulus to obtain a perceptually achromatic surface. We used simulated black body radiations of 3000 (or 4000), 6500, and 20000 K as test illuminants. The results showed that the luminance balance of surfaces with no chromaticity shift had clear effects on the observer’s achromatic setting, which was consistent with our hypothesis on estimating the scene illuminant based on optimal colors.

© 2012 Optical Society of America

OCIS Codes
(330.1690) Vision, color, and visual optics : Color
(330.1720) Vision, color, and visual optics : Color vision

ToC Category:
Color in natural or complex scenes

History
Original Manuscript: September 1, 2011
Revised Manuscript: November 11, 2011
Manuscript Accepted: November 14, 2011
Published: January 19, 2012

Virtual Issues
Vol. 7, Iss. 4 Virtual Journal for Biomedical Optics

Citation
Keiji Uchikawa, Kazuho Fukuda, Yusuke Kitazawa, and Donald I. A. MacLeod, "Estimating illuminant color based on luminance balance of surfaces," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 29, A133-A143 (2012)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josaa/abstract.cfm?URI=josaa-29-2-A133


Sort:  Author  |  Year  |  Journal  |  Reset

References

  1. D. H. Foster, “Color constancy,” Vis. Res. 51, 674–700 (2011). [CrossRef]
  2. G. Buchsbaum, “A spatial processor model for object colour perception,” J. Franklin Inst. 310, 1–26 (1980). [CrossRef]
  3. E. H. Land, “Recent advances in Retinex theory,” Vis. Res. 26, 7–21 (1986). [CrossRef]
  4. R. Brown, “Background and illuminants: The yin and yang of colour constancy,” in Colour Perception: Mind and the Physical World, R. Mausfeld and D. Heyer, eds. (Oxford University, 2003).
  5. J. Golz and D. I. A. MacLeod, “Influence of scene statistics on colour constancy,” Nature 415, 637–640 (2002). [CrossRef]
  6. D. L. Ruderman, T. W. Cronin, and C. Chiao, “Statistics of cone responses to natural images: implication for visual coding,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 15, 2036–2045 (1998). [CrossRef]
  7. L. T. Maloney and B. A. Wandell, “Color constancy: a method for recovering surface spectral reflectance,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 3, 29–33 (1986). [CrossRef]
  8. D. H. Brainard and W. T. Freeman, “Bayesian color constancy,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 14, 1393–1411 (1997). [CrossRef]
  9. D. A. Forsyth, “A novel algorithm for color constancy,” Int. J. Comput. Vis. 5, 5–36 (1990). [CrossRef]
  10. G. D. Finalayson, P. M. Hubel, and S. Hordley, “Color by correlation,” in Proceedings of the Fifth Color Imaging Conference (Society for Imaging Science and Technology, 1997), pp. 6–11.
  11. G. Wyszecki and W. S. Stiles, Color Science, 2nd ed. (Wiley, 1982).
  12. J. J. Koenderink and A. J. van Doorn, “Perspectives on colour space,” in Colour Perception: Mind and the Physical World, D. Mausfeld and D. Heyer, eds. (Oxford University, 2003).
  13. D. B. Judd, “Hue saturation and lightness of surface colors with chromatic illumination,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 30, 2–32 (1940). [CrossRef]
  14. A. Gilchrist, Seeing Black and White (Oxford University, 2006).
  15. E. H. Land and J. J. McCann, “Lightness and Retinex theory,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 61, 1–11 (1971). [CrossRef]
  16. A. Stockman, D. I. A. MacLeod, and N. E. Johnson, “Spectral sensitivities of the human cones,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 10, 2491–2621 (1993). [CrossRef]
  17. D. I. A. MacLeod and R. M. Boynton, “Chromaticity diagram showing cone excitation by stimuli of equal luminance,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 69, 1183–1186 (1979). [CrossRef]
  18. K. Uchikawa, K. Koida, T. Meguro, Y. Yamauchi, and I. Kuriki, “Brightness, not luminance, determines transition from the surface-color to the aperture-color mode for colored lights,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 18, 737–746 (2001). [CrossRef]
  19. J. M. Speigle and D. H. Brainard, “Luminosity thresholds: effects of test chromaticity and ambient illumination,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 13, 436–451 (1996). [CrossRef]
  20. S. Tominaga, S. Ebisui, and B. A. Wandell, “Scene illuminant classification: brighter is better,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 18, 55–64 (2001). [CrossRef]
  21. To further examine this point, it may be useful to note the luminance balances actually used in experiment 4. Under the 4000 K illuminant, surface chromaticities were shifted redward, so that to achieve an L, M, S average equal to the equal energy white, the luminance balance had to be adjusted by dimming the R colors, yielding an R∶B luminance ratio of 1∶3.7; this is much lower than the ratio (1∶0.69) characteristic of optimal colors of those chromaticities under 4000 K (and slightly lower even than the ratio (1∶2.39) that we obtain for optimal colors of those chromaticities under 20000 K). Similarly, the R∶B luminance ratio under our 20000 K illuminant condition (R∶B=1∶0.149) was far higher than would be appropriate for optimal colors of that chromaticity under 20000 K (R∶B=1∶1.123); it was close to, but still more extreme than, that of such optimal colors observed under 4000 K (R∶B=1∶0.339).

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.


« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited