OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

| OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION

  • Vol. 1, Iss. 7 — Jul. 1, 1984
  • pp: 775–782

Physical limits of acuity and hyperacuity

Wilson S. Geisler  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA A, Vol. 1, Issue 7, pp. 775-782 (1984)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.1.000775


View Full Text Article

Enhanced HTML    Acrobat PDF (1164 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

An ideal detector is derived for the discrimination of arbitrary stimuli in the two-alternative forced-choice paradigm. The ideal detector’s performance is assumed to be limited only by quantal fluctuations, the optics of the eye, and the size and spacing of the receptors in the retinal mosaic. Detailed predictions are presented for two-point acuity and hyperacuity tasks. The ideal detector’s two-point resolution, over a wide range of luminances, is approximately 10 times worse than its two-point vernier acuity or separation discrimination. Furthermore, two-point resolution is shown to vary in proportion to the −¼ power of spot intensity, but vernier acuity and separation discrimination vary in proportion to the −½ power of spot intensity. It is shown that this ideal detector can be implemented by the use of appropriately shaped receptive fields. The derivation provides a simple way to determine the shapes of these optimal receptive fields for arbitrary stimuli. The sensitivities of real (human) and ideal detectors are compared.

© 1984 Optical Society of America

History
Original Manuscript: October 26, 1983
Manuscript Accepted: March 13, 1984
Published: July 1, 1984

Citation
Wilson S. Geisler, "Physical limits of acuity and hyperacuity," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1, 775-782 (1984)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josaa/abstract.cfm?URI=josaa-1-7-775


Sort:  Author  |  Year  |  Journal  |  Reset  

References

  1. G. Westheimer, “The spatial sense of the eye,” Invest. Ophthalmol. 18, 893–912 (1979).
  2. T. Y. Baker, G. B. Bryan, “Errors of observation,” in Proceedings of the Optical Convention (Hodder and Stroughton, London, 1912).
  3. R. N. Berry, “Quantitative relations among vernier, real depth, and stereoscopic depth acuities,” J. Exp. Psychol. 38, 708–721 (1948). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. G. Westheimer, S. P. McKee, “Spatial configurations for visual hyperacuity,” Vision Res. 17, 941–947 (1977). [CrossRef]
  5. H. J. Howard, “A test for the judgment of distance,” Am. J. Opthalmol 2, 656–675 (1919).
  6. G. Westheimer, S. P. McKee, “Stereoscopic acuity with defocused and spatially filtered retinal images,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 70, 772–778 (1980). [CrossRef]
  7. G. Westheimer, “Visual hyperacuity,” in Progress In Sensory Physiology, Vol. 1, H. Autrum, D. Ottoson, E. R. Perl, R. F. Schmidt, eds. (Springer-Verlag, New York, 1981), pp. 1–30. [CrossRef]
  8. J. J. Gibson, The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems (Houghton-Mifflin, Boston, 1966).
  9. C. H. Longuet-Higgins, K. Prazdny, “The interpretation of a moving retinal image,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 208, 385–397 (1980). [CrossRef]
  10. W. F. Clocksin, “Perception of surface slant and edge labels from optical flow: a computational approach,” Perception 9, 252–267 (1980). [CrossRef]
  11. D. T. Marr, T. Poggio, “A computational theory of human stereo vision,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 204, 301–328 (1979). [CrossRef]
  12. J. Mayhew, “The interpretation of stereo-disparity information: the computation of surface orientation and depth,” Perception 11, 387–403 (1982). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. C. H. Longuet-Higgins, “The role of the vertical dimension in stereoscopic vision,” Perception 11, 377–386 (1982). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. F. W. Campbell, R. W. Gubisch, “Optical quality of the human eye,” J. Physiol. (London) 186, 558–578 (1966).
  15. W. H. Miller, “Interocular filters,” in Handbook of Sensory Physiology, H. Autrum, R. Jung, W. R. Loewenstein, H. L. Tueber, D. M. Mackay, eds. (Springer-Verlag, Berlin, 1979), Vol. VII/6a, pp. 70–135.
  16. G. Wyszecki, W. S. Stiles, Color Science (Wiley, New York, 1967).
  17. D. M. Green, J. A. Swets, Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics (Wiley, New York, 1966; Krieger, Melbourne, Fla., 1974).
  18. G. Westheimer, S. P. McKee, “Integration regions for visual hyperacuity,” Vision Res. 17, 89–93 (1977). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. W. P. Tanner, R. C. Clark-Jones, “The ideal sensor system as approached through statistical decision theory and the theory of signal detectability,” in Vision Research Problems, A. Morrow, E. P. Horne, eds., NAS–NRC Publ. No. 712 (National Academy of Sciences, Washington, D.C., 1960).
  20. A. Rose, “The relative sensitivities of television pick-up tubes, photographic film, and the human eye,” Proc. IRE 30, 293–300 (1942). [CrossRef]
  21. H. de Vries, “The quantum character of light and its bearing upon the threshold of vision, the differential sensitivity and acuity of the eye,” Physica 10, 553–564 (1943). [CrossRef]
  22. J. Yellot, “Spectral consequences of photoreceptor sampling in the rhesus retina,” Science 22, 382–385 (1983). [CrossRef]
  23. S. Shlaer, “The relation between visual acuity and illumination,” J. Gen. Physiol. 21, 165–188 (1937). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. F. L. Van Nes, M. A. Bouman, “Spatial modulation transfer in the human eye,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 57, 401–406 (1967). [CrossRef]
  25. L. A. Riggs, “Visual acuity,” in Vision and Visual Perception, C. H. Graham, ed. (Wiley, New York, 1966), pp. 321–349.
  26. F. W. Campbell, C. E. Legge, “Displacement detection in human vision,” Vision Res. 21, 205–214 (1981). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. H. B. Barlow, “Increment thresholds at low intensities considered as signal/noise discriminations,” J. Physiol. (London) 136, 469–488 (1957).
  28. M. C. Teich, P. R. Prucnal, G. Vannucci, M. E. Breton, W. J. McGill, “Multiplication noise in the human visual system at threshold. 1. Quantum fluctuations and minimum detectable energy,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 72, 419–431 (1982). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. H. B. Barlow, “Retinal and central factors in human vision limited by noise,” in Photoreception in Vertebrates, H. B. Barlow, P. Fatt, eds. (Academic, New York, 1977), Chap. 19, pp. 337–358.
  30. W. S. Geisler, “Effects of bleaches and backgrounds on the flash response of the cone system,” J. Physiol. (London) 312, 413–434 (1981).
  31. W. S. Geisler, “Mechanisms of visual sensitivity: backgrounds and early dark adaptation,” Vision Res. 23, 1423–1432. [PubMed]
  32. H. B. Barlow, “Reconstructing the visual image in space and time,” Nature 79, 189–190 (1979). [CrossRef]
  33. H. B. Barlow, “Critical limiting factors in the design of the eye and visual cortex,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 212, 1–34 (1981). [CrossRef]
  34. R. J. Watt, M. J. Morgan, “Mechanisms responsible for the assessment of visual location: theory and evidence,” Vision Res. 23, 97–109 (1983). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Figures 5 and 7 show, for the present ideal detector, that two-point resolution follows an inverse fourth-root law and that two-point separation discrimination and vernier acuity follow an inverse square-root law. It will be proved in a subsequent paper that these relations are not true just for point sources but hold for essentially arbitrarily shaped stimuli. Thus, for any stimulus shape, the measurement of resolution and some hyperacuity as a function of luminance will provide a strong, parameter-free test of the quantum-fluctuations hypothesis.

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.


« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited