OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

| OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION

  • Vol. 15, Iss. 9 — Sep. 1, 1998
  • pp: 2257–2262

Interocular suppression of a half-occluded region of stereoscopic images

Masaki Emoto and Tetsuo Mitsuhashi  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA A, Vol. 15, Issue 9, pp. 2257-2262 (1998)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.15.002257


View Full Text Article

Acrobat PDF (274 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

We assessed the detectability d of a monocular small gray dot target presented on a half-occluded region of stereoscopic three-dimensional background images by comparing it with that on a two-dimensional (2D) region. For our experiments we used a typical two-alternative temporal forced-choice procedure, in which the target was presented in one of two temporal intervals for ~67 ms, and observers selected the interval they believed to have contained the target by pressing the corresponding key. To vary target signal intensity, we changed the target contrast against the background. According to signal-detection theory, we converted the percent-correct data to detectability d and found that the relationship between d and the contrast of the target followed Legge’s equation. We used Legge’s equation to calculate the contrast threshold and found that the contrast threshold of the target on the half-occluded region was higher than that on the 2D region. This elevation of contrast threshold indicates that interocular suppression of the half-occluded region occurs more frequently than that of the 2D region. We also refer to the monocular performance of the human visual system.

© 1998 Optical Society of America

OCIS Codes
(330.1400) Vision, color, and visual optics : Vision - binocular and stereopsis
(330.7310) Vision, color, and visual optics : Vision

Citation
Masaki Emoto and Tetsuo Mitsuhashi, "Interocular suppression of a half-occluded region of stereoscopic images," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 15, 2257-2262 (1998)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josaa/abstract.cfm?URI=josaa-15-9-2257


Sort:  Author  |  Year  |  Journal  |  Reset

References

  1. C. M. Aschenbrenner, “Problems in getting information into and out of air photographs,” Photogramm. Eng. 20, 398–401 (1954).
  2. B. Julesz, “Binocular depth perception of computer generated patterns,” Bell Syst. Tech. J. 39, 1125–1162 (1960).
  3. B. Julesz, “Binocular depth perception without familiarity cues,” Science 145, 356–362 (1964).
  4. S. Shimojo and K. Nakayama, “Real world occlusion constraints and binocular rivalry,” Vision Res. 30, 69–80 (1990).
  5. S. Shimojo and K. Nakayama, “Interocularly unpaired zones escape local binocular matching,” Vision Res. 34, 1875–1881 (1994).
  6. H. R. Blackwell, “Psychophysical thresholds: experimental studies of methods of measurement,” University of Michigan Engineering Research Bulletin No. 36 (University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Mich., 1953).
  7. J. S. Lukaszewski and D. N. Elliott, “Auditory threshold as a function of forced-choice techniques, feedback, and motivation,” J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 34, 223–228 (1962).
  8. J. A. Swets, ed., Signal Detection and Recognition by Human Observers: Contemporary Readings (Wiley, New York, 1964).
  9. G. E. Legge, “Binocular contrast summation-1,” Vision Res. 24, 373–383 (1983).
  10. M. Emoto and T. Mitsuhashi, “Perception of edge sharpness in three dimensional images,” in Human Vision, Visual Processing, and Digital Display VI, B. E. Rogowitz and J. P. Allebach, eds., Proc. SPIE 2411, 250–261 (1995).
  11. W. Makous, D. Teller, and R. Boothe, “Binocular interaction in the dark,” Vision Res. 16, 473–476 (1976).
  12. B. Gillam and E. Borsting, “The role of monocular regions in stereoscopic displays,” Perception 17, 603–608 (1988).
  13. L. Liu, S. B. Stevenson, and C. M. Schor, “Quantitative stereoscopic depth without binocular correspondence,” Nature (London) 367, 66–69 (1994).

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.


Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited