OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

| OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION

  • Vol. 19, Iss. 10 — Oct. 1, 2002
  • pp: 2144–2153

Stereomotion is processed by the third-order motion system: reply to comment on “Three-systems theory of human visual motion perception: review and update”

Zhong-Lin Lu and George Sperling  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA A, Vol. 19, Issue 10, pp. 2144-2153 (2002)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.19.002144


View Full Text Article

Acrobat PDF (469 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

Two theories are considered to account for the perception of motion of depth-defined objects in random-dot stereograms (stereomotion). In the Lu–Sperling three-motion-systems theory [J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 18, 2331 (2001)], stereomotion is perceived by the third-order motion system, which detects the motion of areas defined as figure (versus ground) in a salience map. Alternatively, in his comment [J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 19, 2142 (2002)], Patterson proposes a low-level motion-energy system dedicated to stereo depth. The critical difference between these theories is the preprocessing (figure–ground based on depth and other cues versus simply stereo depth) rather than the motion-detection algorithm itself (because the motion-extraction algorithm for third-order motion is undetermined). Furthermore, the ability of observers to perceive motion in alternating feature displays in which stereo depth alternates with other features such as texture orientation indicates that the third-order motion system can perceive stereomotion. This reduces the stereomotion question to “Is it third-order alone or third-order plus dedicated depth-motion processing?” Two new experiments intended to support the dedicated depth-motion processing theory are shown here to be perfectly accounted for by third-order motion, as are many older experiments that have previously been shown to be consistent with third-order motion. Cyclopean and rivalry images are shown to be a likely confound in stereomotion studies, rivalry motion being as strong as stereomotion. The phase dependence of superimposed same-direction stereomotion stimuli, rivalry stimuli, and isoluminant color stimuli indicates that these stimuli are processed in the same (third-order) motion system. The phase-dependence paradigm [Lu and Sperling, Vision Res. 35, 2697 (1995)] ultimately can resolve the question of which types of signals share a single motion detector. All the evidence accumulated so far is consistent with the three-motion-systems theory.

© 2002 Optical Society of America

OCIS Codes
(330.4150) Vision, color, and visual optics : Motion detection

Citation
Zhong-Lin Lu and George Sperling, "Stereomotion is processed by the third-order motion system: reply to comment on “Three-systems theory of human visual motion perception: review and update”," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 19, 2144-2153 (2002)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josaa/abstract.cfm?URI=josaa-19-10-2144


Sort:  Author  |  Year  |  Journal  |  Reset

References

  1. Z.-L. Lu and G. Sperling, “The functional architecture of human visual motion perception,” Vision Res. 35, 2697–2722 (1995).
  2. Z.-L. Lu and G. Sperling, “Three-systems theory of human visual motion perception: review and update,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 18, 2331–2370 (2001).
  3. R. Patterson, “Stereoscopic (cyclopean) motion sensing,” Vision Res. 39, 3329–3345 (1999).
  4. H. J. Kim, Z.-L. Lu, and G. Sperling, “Rivalry motion versus depth motion,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. ARVO Suppl. 42, 3947 (2001).
  5. R. Patterson, “Three-systems theory of human visual motion perception: review and update: comment,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 19, 2142–2143 (2002).
  6. A. T. Smith and N. E. Scott-Samuel, “Stereoscopic and contrast-defined motion in human vision,” Proc. R. Soc. London Ser. B 265, 1573–1581 (1998).
  7. H. Ito, “Two processes in stereoscopic apparent motion,” Vision Res. 39, 2739–2748 (1999).
  8. J. P. H. van Santen and G. Sperling, “Temporal covariance model of human motion perception,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1, 451–473 (1984).
  9. E. H. Adelson and J. R. Bergen, “Spatio-temporal energy models for the perception of apparent motion,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2, 284–299 (1985).
  10. C. Chubb and G. Sperling, “Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 5, 1986–2006 (1988).
  11. Z.-L. Lu and G. Sperling, “Attention-generated apparent motion,” Nature 377, 237–239 (1995).
  12. E. Blaser, G. Sperling, and Z.-L. Lu, “Measuring the amplification of attention,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96, 11681–11686 (1999).
  13. P. Cavanagh, “Attention-based motion perception,” Science 257, 1563–1565 (1992).
  14. Z.-L. Lu, L. A. Lesmes, and G. Sperling, “Perceptual motion standstill in rapidly moving chromatic displays,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 96, 15374–15379 (1999).
  15. P. Cavanagh, M.-A. Henaff, F. Michel, T. Landis, T. Troscianki, and J. Intriligator, “Complete sparing of high-contrast color input to motion perception in cortical color blindness,” Nat. Neurosci. 1, 242–247 (1998).
  16. Q. Zaidi and S. J. DeBonet, “Motion energy versus position tracking: spatial, temporal and chromatic parameters,” Vision Res. 40, 3613–3635 (2000).
  17. P. Cavanagh, M. Arguin, and M. von Gruenau, “Interattribute apparent motion,” Vision Res. 29, 1197–1204 (1989).
  18. R. Patterson, M. Donnelly, R. E. Phinney, M. Nawrot, A. Whiting, and T. Eyle, “Speed discrimination of stereoscopic (cyclopean) motion,” Vision Res. 37, 871–878 (1997).
  19. S. Nishida, T. Ledgeway, and M. Edwards, “Dual multiple-scale processing for motion in the human visual system,” Vision Res. 37, 2685–2698 (1997).
  20. Z.-L. Lu, G. Sperling, and J. Beck, “Selective adaptation of three motion systems,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. ARVO Suppl. 38, 237 (1997).
  21. W. A. Phillips, “On the distinction between sensory storage and short-term visual memory,” Percept. Psychophys. 16, 283–290 (1974).
  22. E. H. Adelson, “Some new motion illusions, and some old ones, analysed in terms of their Fourier components,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. ARVO Suppl. 34, 144 (1982).
  23. M. A. Georgeson and T. M. Shackleton, “Monocular motion sensing, binocular motion perception,” Vision Res. 29, 1511–1523 (1989).
  24. A. T. Smith, “Correspondence-based and energy-based detection of second-order motion in human vision,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 11, 1940–1948 (1994).
  25. P. Burt and B. Julesz, “A disparity gradient limit for binocular fusion,” Science 208, 615–617 (1980).
  26. W. Reichardt, “Autocorrelation, a principle for the evaluation of sensory information by the central nervous system,” in Sensory Communication, W. A. Rosenblith, ed. (Wiley, New York, 1961), pp. 303–317.
  27. S. M. Anstis, “Phi movement as a subtraction process,” Vision Res. 10, 1411–1430 (1970).
  28. C. Chubb and G. Sperling, “Two motion perception mechanisms revealed through distance-driven reversal of apparent motion,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 86, 2985–2989 (1989).
  29. T. V. Papathomas, A. Gorea, and C. Chubb, “Precise assessment of the mean effective luminance of texture patches: an approach based on reverse-phi motion,” Vision Res. 36, 3775–3784 (1996).
  30. Z.-L. Lu and G. Sperling, “Second-order reversed phi,” Percept. Psychophys. 61, 1075–1088 (1999).
  31. C. Chubb and G. Sperling, “Texture quilts: basic tools for studying motion-from-texture,” J. Math. Psychol. 35, 411–442 (1991).
  32. J. P. H. van Santen and G. Sperling, “Elaborated Reichardt detectors,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2, 300–321 (1985).
  33. R. P. O’Shea and R. Blake, “Depth without disparity in random-dot stereograms,” Percept. Psychophys. 42, 205–214 (1987).
  34. C. H. Tseng, H. Kim, J. L. Gobell, Z.-L. Lu, and G. Sperling, “Motion standstill in rapidly moving stereoptic depth displays,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. ARVO Suppl. 42, S504 (2001).

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.


« Previous Article

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited