OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

| OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION

  • Vol. 19, Iss. 8 — Aug. 1, 2002
  • pp: 1475–1483

Adaptation of spatiotemporal mechanisms by increment and decrement stimuli

Todd J. Purkiss and Paul J. DeMarco, Jr.  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA A, Vol. 19, Issue 8, pp. 1475-1483 (2002)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.19.001475


View Full Text Article

Enhanced HTML    Acrobat PDF (410 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

Sawtooth modulation has been used in the past to examine visual sensitivity to luminance increments and decrements. The threshold elevation caused by adaptation depends on the spatial profile of the stimulus field and the polarities of the adaptation and test stimuli. We hypothesized that the adaptation effects reflect a change in the sensitivity of the spatiotemporal channels that detect the stimuli. We used a 2-deg disk centered in a larger surround field. Five levels of contrast between the test field and surround were investigated: equiluminant, three intermediate levels, and dark. At each contrast, observers adapted for 5 s to 2-Hz sawtooth modulation (rapid-on or rapid-off). Immediately after adaptation, thresholds were measured for detection of a single cycle of either a rapid-on or a rapid-off waveform. Varying the contrast of the surround affected observers’ sensitivity to the polarity of the sawtooth stimulus to the extent that the pattern of sensitivity with the equiluminant surround was the opposite of that with the dark surround. To examine temporal factors, we measured thresholds for slow (500-ms ramps) and fast (8.3-ms pulses) test stimuli. The adaptation effect was preserved with the ramp stimuli but not with the pulse stimuli. Blurring the edge between the test and surround fields in the equiluminant surround condition raised thresholds for all sawtooth test stimuli, suggesting that spatiotemporal channels sensitive to high spatial frequencies and low temporal frequencies facilitate detection in that condition. These findings suggest that adaptation to sawtooth modulation can differentially effect the sensitivity of ON and OFF pathways, but the relative desensitization of each pathway depends on an interaction with the adaptation state of spatiotemporal channels that are involved in detection.

© 2002 Optical Society of America

OCIS Codes
(330.1880) Vision, color, and visual optics : Detection
(330.5510) Vision, color, and visual optics : Psychophysics
(330.7320) Vision, color, and visual optics : Vision adaptation

History
Original Manuscript: September 10, 2001
Revised Manuscript: February 4, 2002
Manuscript Accepted: February 5, 2002
Published: August 1, 2002

Citation
Todd J. Purkiss and Paul J. DeMarco, "Adaptation of spatiotemporal mechanisms by increment and decrement stimuli," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 19, 1475-1483 (2002)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josaa/abstract.cfm?URI=josaa-19-8-1475


Sort:  Author  |  Year  |  Journal  |  Reset  

References

  1. M. Hanly, D. M. MacKay, “Polarity-sensitive perceptual adaptation to temporal sawtooth modulation of luminance,” Exp. Brain Res. 35, 37–46 (1979). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. J. Krauskopf, “Discrimination and detection of changes in luminance,” Vision Res. 20, 671–677 (1980). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. J. K. Krauskopf, Q. Zaidi, “Induced desensitization,” Vision Res. 26, 759–762 (1986). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. T. J. Purkiss, A. Hughes, P. J. DeMarco, “Processing of scotopic increments and decrements,” Visual Neurosci. 18, 119–125 (2001). [CrossRef]
  5. L. Roveri, P. J. Demarco, G. G. Celesia, “An electrophysiological metric of activity within the ON- and OFF-pathways in humans,” Vision Res. 37, 669–674 (1997). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. D. H. Kelly, “Flickering patterns and lateral inhibition,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 59, 1361–1365 (1969). [CrossRef]
  7. J. A. J. Roufs, “Dynamic properties of vision—I. Experimental relationships between flicker and flash thresholds,” Vision Res. 12, 261–278 (1972). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. U. T. Keesey, “Variables determining flicker sensitivity in small fields,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 60, 390–398 (1970). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. A. Eisner, “Nonmonotonic effects of test illuminance on flicker detection: a study of foveal light adaptation with annular surrounds,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 11, 33–47 (1994). [CrossRef]
  10. A. Eisner, A. G. Shapiro, J. A. Middleton, “Equivalence between temporal frequency and modulation depth for flicker response suppression: analysis of a three-process model of visual adaptation,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 15, 1987–2002 (1998). [CrossRef]
  11. A. Eisner, J. R. Samples, “Flicker sensitivity and cardiovascular function in healthy middle-aged people,” Arch. Ophthalmol. 118, 1049–1055 (2000). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. B. Spehar, Q. Zaidi, “Surround effects on the shape of the temporal contrast-sensitivity function,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 14, 2517–2525 (1997). [CrossRef]
  13. M. Aguilar, W. S. Stiles, “Saturation of the rod mechanism of the retina at high levels of illumination,” Opt. Acta 1, 59–65 (1954). [CrossRef]
  14. R. W. Bowen, J. Pokorny, V. C. Smith, “Sawtooth contrast sensitivity: decrements have the edge,” Vision Res. 29, 1501–1509 (1989). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. R. W. Bowen, J. Pokorny, V. C. Smith, M. A. Fowler, “Sawtooth contrast sensitivity: effects of mean illumi-nance and low temporal frequencies,” Vision Res. 32, 1239–1247 (1992). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. P. J. DeMarco, V. S. Smith, J. Pokorny, “Effect of sawtooth polarity on chromatic and luminance detection,” Visual Neurosci. 11, 491–499 (1994). [CrossRef]
  17. P. Whittle, “Brightness, discriminability and the ‘crispening effect’,” Vision Res. 32, 1493–1507 (1992). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. A. J. Anderson, A. J. Vingrys, “Multiple processes mediate flicker sensitivity,” Vision Res. 41, 2449–2455 (2001). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. A. J. Anderson, A. J. Vingrys, “Interactions between flicker thresholds and luminance pedestals,” Vision Res. 40, 2579–2588 (2000). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. J. K. Krauskopf, Q. Zaidi, M. B. Mandler, “Mechanisms of simultaneous color induction,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 3, 1752–1757 (1986). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. J. Krauskopf, D. R. Williams, D. W. Heeley, “Cardinal directions of color space,” Vision Res. 22, 1123–1131 (1982). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. P. J. DeMarco, V. C. Smith, J. Pokorny, “Effect of sawtooth polarity on chromatic and luminance detection,” Visual Neurosci. 11, 491–499 (1994). [CrossRef]
  23. A. Eisner, “Suppression of flicker response with increasing test illuminance: roles of temporal waveform, modulation depth, and frequency,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 12, 214–224 (1995). [CrossRef]
  24. L. Roveri, P. J. DeMarco, G. C. Celesia, “An electrophysiological metric of activity within the ON- and OFF-pathways in humans,” Vision Res. 37, 669–674 (1997). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. K. Shinomori, L. Spillmann, J. S. Werner, “Off responses from s-cones: temporal signals vary with post-receptoral mechanisms,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. Suppl. 38, 4163 (1997).
  26. T. J. Purkiss, A. Hughes, P. J. DeMarco, “Processing of scotopic increments and decrements,” Visual Neurosci. 18, 119–125 (2001). [CrossRef]
  27. K. R. Alexander, C. S. Barnes, G. A. Fishman, “High-frequency attenuation of the cone ERG and ON-response deficits in X-linked retinoschisis,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. 42, 2094–2101 (2001).
  28. K. R. Alexander, G. A. Fishman, C. S. Barnes, S. Grover, “On-response deficit in the electroretinogram of the cone system in X-linked retinoschisis,” Invest. Ophthalmol. Visual Sci. 42, 453–459 (2001).
  29. P. E. King-Smith, J. J. Kulikowski, “Pattern and flicker detection analysed by subthreshold summation,” J. Physiol. (London) 249, 519–548 (1975).
  30. J. J. Kulikowski, D. J. Tolhurst, “Psychophysical evidence for sustained and transient detectors in human vision,” J. Physiol. (London) 232, 149–162 (1973).
  31. D. J. Tolhurst, “Separate channels for the analysis of the shape and the movement of moving visual stimulus,” J. Physiol. (London) 231, 385–402 (1973).
  32. E. Kaplan, B. B. Lee, R. M. Shapley, “New views of primate retinal function,” in Progress in Retinal Research, N. Osborne, J. Chader, eds. (Pergamon, Oxford, UK, 1990), pp. 273–336.
  33. P. Lennie, “Parallel visual pathways: a review,” Vision Res. 20, 561–594 (1980). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. J. Kremers, B. B. Lee, J. Pokorny, V. C. Smith, “Responses of macaque ganglion cells and human observers to compound periodic waveforms,” Vision Res. 33, 1997–2011 (1993). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. S. M. Anstis, “Visual adaptation to gradual change of intensity,” Science 155, 710–712 (1967). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. K. Arnold, S. Anstis, “Properties of the visual channels that underlie adaptation to gradual change of luminance,” Vision Res. 33, 47–54 (1993). [CrossRef] [PubMed]

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.


« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited