OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

| OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION

  • Editor: Stephen A. Burns
  • Vol. 24, Iss. 7 — Jul. 1, 2007
  • pp: 1830–1841

Effects of surround articulation on lightness depend on the spatial arrangement of the articulated region

Iris K. Zemach and Michael E. Rudd  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA A, Vol. 24, Issue 7, pp. 1830-1841 (2007)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.24.001830


View Full Text Article

Enhanced HTML    Acrobat PDF (376 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

We investigated the effect of surround articulation on the perceived lightness of a target disk. Surround articulation was manipulated by varying either the number of wedges in a surround consisting of wedges of alternating luminance or the number of checks in a surround consisting of a radial checkerboard pattern. In most conditions, increased articulation caused incremental targets to appear lighter and decremental targets to appear darker. But increasing the surround articulation in a way that did not increase the number of target-coaligned edges in the display did not affect the target lightness. We propose that the effects of surround articulation depend on the relationship between the orientations and contrast polarities of the target edges and those of edges present within the surround.

© 2007 Optical Society of America

OCIS Codes
(330.1720) Vision, color, and visual optics : Color vision
(330.5020) Vision, color, and visual optics : Perception psychology
(330.5510) Vision, color, and visual optics : Psychophysics
(330.7310) Vision, color, and visual optics : Vision

ToC Category:
Vision and Color

History
Original Manuscript: August 3, 2006
Revised Manuscript: January 12, 2007
Manuscript Accepted: January 25, 2007
Published: June 13, 2007

Virtual Issues
Vol. 2, Iss. 8 Virtual Journal for Biomedical Optics

Citation
Iris K. Zemach and Michael E. Rudd, "Effects of surround articulation on lightness depend on the spatial arrangement of the articulated region," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 24, 1830-1841 (2007)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josaa/abstract.cfm?URI=josaa-24-7-1830


Sort:  Author  |  Year  |  Journal  |  Reset  

References

  1. H. von Helmholtz, Helmholtz's Treatise on Physiological Optics (Dover, 1962) (translation of 1896 German edition).
  2. A. Gilchrist, C. Kossyfidis, F. Bonato, T. Agostini, J. Cataliotti, X. Li, B. Spehar, V. Annan, and E. Economou, "An anchoring theory of lightness perception," Psychol. Rev. 106, 795-834 (1999). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. E. G. Heinemann, "Simultaneous brightness induction as a function of inducing and test-field luminances," J. Exp. Psychol. 50, 89-96 (1955). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. E. G. Heinemann, "Simultaneous brightness induction," in Handbook of Sensory Physiology, D.Jameson and L.Hurvich, eds. (Springer-Verlag, 1972), pp. 146-169.
  5. J. M. Kraft and D. H. Brainard, "Mechanisms of color constancy under nearly natural viewing," Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96, 307-312 (1999). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. J. A. Schirillo and S. K. Shevell, "Brightness contrast from inhomogeneous surrounds," Vision Res. 36, 1783-1796 (1996). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. E. H. Adelson, "Perceptual organization and the judgment of brightness," Science 262, 2042-2044 (1993). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. H. Boyaci, L. T. Maloney, and S. Hersh, "The effect of perceived surface orientation on perceived surface albedo in binocularly viewed scenes," J. Vision 3, 541-553 (2003). [CrossRef]
  9. K. J. Linnell and D. H. Foster, "Scene articulation: dependence of illuminant estimates on number of surfaces," Perception 31, 151-159 (2002). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. A. D. Logvinenko, "Articulation in the context of edge classification," Perception 31, 201-207 (2002). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. D. Bindman and C. Chubb, "Brightness assimilation in bullseye displays," Vision Res. 44, 309-319 (2004). [CrossRef]
  12. J. A. Schirillo, "Surround articulation. II. Lightness judgments," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 16, 804-811 (1999). [CrossRef]
  13. J. A. Schirillo, "Surround articulation. I. Brightness judgments," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 16, 793-803 (1999). [CrossRef]
  14. N. Bruno, P. Bernardis, and J. Schirillo, "Lightness, equivalent backgrounds, and anchoring," Percept. Psychophys. 59, 643-654 (1997). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. R. O. Brown and D. I. MacLeod, "Color appearance depends on the variance of surround colors," Curr. Biol. 7, 844-849 (1997). [CrossRef]
  16. D. Bindman and C. Chubb, "Mechanisms of contrast induction in heterogeneous displays," Vision Res. 44, 1601-1613 (2004). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. H. Helson, "Studies of anomalous contrast and assimilation," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 53, 179-184 (1963). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. A. Valberg and B. Lange-Malecki, "'Colour constancy' in Mondrian patterns: a partial cancellation of physical chromaticity shifts by simultaneous contrast," Vision Res. 30, 371-380 (1990). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. H. Helson, "Some factors and implications of color constancy," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 33, 555-567 (1943). [CrossRef]
  20. H. Helson, Adaptation-Level Theory (Harper & Row, 1964).
  21. D. Katz, The World of Colour (Trench, Trubner, 1935).
  22. A. Gelb, "Die 'Farbenkonstanz' der Sehdinge [Color constancy of visual objects]," Handbuch der normalen und pathologishen Physiologie, W.von Bethe, ed. (Springer, 1929), Vol. 12, 594—678. [CrossRef]
  23. L. E. Arend and B. Spehar, "Lightness, brightness, and brightness contrast: 2. Reflectance variation," Percept. Psychophys. 54, 457-468 (1993). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. L. E. Arend and B. Spehar, "Lightness, brightness, and brightness contrast: 1. Illuminance variation," Percept. Psychophys. 54, 446-456 (1993). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. C. Chubb, G. Sperling, and J. A. Solomon, "Texture interactions determine perceived contrast," Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 86, 9631-9635 (1989). [CrossRef]
  26. S. K. Shevell and J. Wei, "Chromatic induction: border contrast or adaptation to surrounding light?" Vision Res. 38, 1561-1566 (1998). [CrossRef]
  27. M. E. Rudd, "Lightness computation by a neural filling-in mechanism," Proc. SPIE 4299, 400-413 (2001). [CrossRef]
  28. M. E. Rudd and K. F. Arrington, "Darkness filling-in: a neural model of darkness induction," Vision Res. 41, 3649-3662 (2001). [CrossRef]
  29. M. E. Rudd and I. K. Zemach, "Quantitative properties of achromatic color induction: an edge integration analysis," Vision Res. 44, 971-981 (2004). [CrossRef]
  30. M. E. Rudd and I. K. Zemach, "The highest luminance anchoring rule in achromatic color perception: some counterexamples and an alternative theory," J. Vision 5, 983-1003 (2005). [CrossRef]
  31. M. E. Rudd and I. K. Zemach, "Contrast polarity and edge integration in achromatic color perception," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 24(8) (2007). [CrossRef]
  32. S. W. Hong and S. K. Shevell, "Brightness induction: unequal spatial integration with increments and decrements," Visual Neurosci. 21, 353-357 (2004). [CrossRef]
  33. S. W. Hong and S. K. Shevell, "Brightness contrast and assimilation from patterned inducing backgrounds," Vision Res. 44, 35-43 (2004). [CrossRef]
  34. A. Kirschmann, "Uber die quantitativen Verhaltnisse des simultanen Helligkeits und Farben-kontrastes," Psychol. Stud. (Wundt) 6, 417-491 (1891).
  35. A. L. Diamond, "Foveal simultaneous brightness contrast as a function of inducing field area," J. Exp. Psychol. 50, 144-152 (1955). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. A. L. Diamond, "Foveal simultaneous brightness contrast as a function of inducing- and test-field luminance," J. Exp. Psychol. 45, 304-314 (1953). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. H. Leibowitz, F. A. Mote, and W. R. Thurlow, "Simultaneous contrast as a function of separation between test and inducing fields," J. Exp. Psychol. 46, 453-456 (1953). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. D. Jameson and L. M. Hurvich, "Complexities of perceived brightness," Science 133, 174-179 (1961). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. J. A. S. Kinney, "Factors affecting induced color," Vision Res. 2, 503-525 (1962). [CrossRef]
  40. T. Oyama and Y. Hsia, "Compensatory hue shifts in simultaneous color contrast as a function of separation between inducing and test fields," J. Exp. Psychol. 71, 405-413 (1966). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. J. Walraven, "Spatial characteristics of chromatic induction; the segregation of lateral effects from straylight artefacts," Vision Res. 13, 1739-1753 (1973). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. H. Wassle and F. Heinrich, "Untersuchungen zum Helligkeitskontrast," Vision Res. 10, 361-373 (1970). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. D. Zavagno, V. Annan, and G. Caputo, "The problem of being white: testing the highest luminance rule," Vision 16, 149-159 (2004).
  44. T. Agostini and A. Galmonte, "Spatial articulation affects lightness," Percept. Psychophys. 61, 1345-1355 (1999). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. L. E. Arend, J. N. Buehler, and G. R. Lockhead, "Difference information in brightness perception," Percept. Psychophys. 9, 367-370 (1971). [CrossRef]
  46. R. Shapley and R. C. Reid, "Contrast and assimilation in the perception of brightness," Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 82, 5983-5986 (1985). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. R. C. Reid, Jr., and R. Shapley, "Brightness induction by local contrast and the spatial dependence of assimilation," Vision Res. 28, 115-132 (1988). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. A. Gilchrist, "Lightness contrast and failures or contrast: a common explanation," Percept. Psychophys. 43, 415-424 (1988). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. S. K. Shevell, I. Holiday, and P. Whittle, "Two separate neural mechanisms of brightness induction," Vision Res. 32, 2331-2340 (1992). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. T. Vladusich, M. P. Lucassen, and F. W. Cornelissen, "Edge integration and the perception of brightness and darkness," J. Vision 6, 1126-1147 (2006). [CrossRef]
  51. M. E. Chevreul, The Principles of Harmony and Contrast of Colours and Their Applications to the Arts (Reinhold, 1967) (translation of the 1839 French edition).
  52. J. W. von Goethe, Theory of Colours (MIT, 1970) (translation of the 1810 German edition).
  53. H. Wallach, "The perception of neutral colors," Sci. Am. 208, 107-116 (1963). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. H. Wallach, "Brightness constancy and the nature of achromatic colors," J. Exp. Psychol. 38, 310-324 (1948). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. H. Wallach, On Perception (Quadrangle/The New York Times Book, 1976).
  56. P. Bressan and R. Actis-Grosso, "Simultaneous lightness contrast with double increments," Perception 30, 889-897 (2001). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. M. E. Rudd, "Edge integration and anchoring in the perception of lightness, brightness, and brightness contrast" (manuscript in preparation, available from the author, mrudd@u.washington.edu).
  58. E. H. Land and J. J. McCann, "Lightness and retinex theory," J. Opt. Soc. Am. 61, 1-11 (1971). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. B. K. P. Horn, "Determining lightness from an image," Comput. Graph. Image Process. 3, 277-299 (1974). [CrossRef]
  60. B. K. P. Horn, "Understanding image intensities," Artif. Intell. 21, 201-231 (1977). [CrossRef]
  61. E. H. Land, "The retinex theory of color vision," Sci. Am. 237, 108-128 (1977). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. E. H. Land, "Recent advances in retinex theory and some implications for cortical computations: color vision and the natural image," Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 80, 5163-5169 (1983). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. E. H. Land, "Recent advances in retinex theory," Vision Res. 26, 7-21 (1986). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. E. H. Land, "An alternative technique for the computation of the designator in the retinex theory of color vision," Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 83, 3078-3080 (1986). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. M. E. Rudd and D. Popa, "Stevens' brightness law, contrast gain control, and edge integration in achromatic color perception: a unified model," submitted to J. Opt. Soc. Am. A.
  66. M. E. Rudd and D. Popa, "A theory of the neural processes underlying edge integration in human lightness perception [abstract]," J. Vision 4, 345a (2004). [CrossRef]
  67. M. E. Rudd and D. Popa, "Edge integration and edge interaction in achromatic color computation [abstract]," J. Vision 4, 79a (2004). [CrossRef]
  68. J. Cataliotti and A. Gilchrist, "Local and global processes in surface lightness perception," Percept. Psychophys. 57, 125-135 (1995). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. C. Hess and H. Pretori, "Quantitative investigation of the lawfulness of simultaneous brightness contrast," Percept. Mot. Skills 31, 947-969 (1970) (translation of 1894 paper by H. R. Flock and J. H. Tenney). [CrossRef]
  70. A. Kozaki, "A further study in the relationship between brightness constancy and contrast," Jap. Psychol. Res. 5, 129-136 (1963).
  71. A. Kozaki, "The effect of co-existent stimuli other than the test stimulus on brightness constancy," Jap. Psychol. Res. 7, 138-147 (1965).
  72. A. Jacobsen and A. Gilchrist, "Hess and Pretori revisited: resolution of some old contradictions," Percept. Psychophys. 43, 7-14 (1988). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. T. Agostini and N. Bruno, "Lightness contrast in CRT and paper-and-illuminant displays," Percept. Psychophys. 58, 250-258 (1996). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. X. Li and A. L. Gilchrist, "Relative area and relative luminance combine to anchor surface lightness values," Percept. Psychophys. 61, 771-785 (1999). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. L. T. Maloney and J. A. Schirillo, "Color constancy, lightness constancy, and the articulation hypothesis," Perception 31, 135-139 (2002). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. J. J. McCann, S. P. McKee, and T. H. Taylor, "Quantitative studies in retinex theory. A comparison between theoretical predictions and observer responses to the 'color mondrian' experiments," Vision Res. 16, 445-458 (1976). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. D. Todorovic, "Lightness and junctions," Perception 26, 379-394 (1997). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. W. Benary, "Beobachtungen zu einem Experiment uber Helligkeitskontrast," Psychol. Forsch. 5, 131-142 (1924). [CrossRef]
  79. T. Agostini and D. R. Proffitt, "Perceptual organization evokes simultaneous lightness contrast," Perception 22, 263-272 (1993). [CrossRef] [PubMed]

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.

Figures

Fig. 1 Fig. 2 Fig. 3
 

« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited