OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

| OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION

  • Editor: Franco Gori
  • Vol. 29, Iss. 2 — Feb. 1, 2012
  • pp: A102–A107

Blue–green color categorization in Mandarin–English speakers

Sophie Wuerger, Kaida Xiao, Dimitris Mylonas, Qingmei Huang, Dimosthenis Karatzas, Emily Hird, and Galina Paramei  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA A, Vol. 29, Issue 2, pp. A102-A107 (2012)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.29.00A102


View Full Text Article

Enhanced HTML    Acrobat PDF (625 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

Observers are faster to detect a target among a set of distracters if the targets and distracters come from different color categories. This cross-boundary advantage seems to be limited to the right visual field, which is consistent with the dominance of the left hemisphere for language processing [Gilbert et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 103, 489 (2006)]. Here we study whether a similar visual field advantage is found in the color identification task in speakers of Mandarin, a language that uses a logographic system. Forty late Mandarin–English bilinguals performed a blue–green color categorization task, in a blocked design, in their first language (L1: Mandarin) or second language (L2: English). Eleven color singletons ranging from blue to green were presented for 160 ms, randomly in the left visual field (LVF) or right visual field (RVF). Color boundary and reaction times (RTs) at the color boundary were estimated in L1 and L2, for both visual fields. We found that the color boundary did not differ between the languages; RTs at the color boundary, however, were on average more than 100 ms shorter in the English compared to the Mandarin sessions, but only when the stimuli were presented in the RVF. The finding may be explained by the script nature of the two languages: Mandarin logographic characters are analyzed visuospatially in the right hemisphere, which conceivably facilitates identification of color presented to the LVF.

© 2012 Optical Society of America

OCIS Codes
(330.1720) Vision, color, and visual optics : Color vision
(330.5510) Vision, color, and visual optics : Psychophysics

ToC Category:
Variations and deficiencies of color vision

History
Original Manuscript: September 12, 2011
Revised Manuscript: November 7, 2011
Manuscript Accepted: November 9, 2011
Published: January 17, 2012

Virtual Issues
Vol. 7, Iss. 4 Virtual Journal for Biomedical Optics

Citation
Sophie Wuerger, Kaida Xiao, Dimitris Mylonas, Qingmei Huang, Dimosthenis Karatzas, Emily Hird, and Galina Paramei, "Blue–green color categorization in Mandarin–English speakers," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 29, A102-A107 (2012)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josaa/abstract.cfm?URI=josaa-29-2-A102


Sort:  Author  |  Year  |  Journal  |  Reset  

References

  1. M. H. Bornstein and M. D. Monroe, “Chromatic information processing: rate depends on stimulus location in the category and psychological complexity,” Psychol. Res. 42, 213–225 (1980). [CrossRef]
  2. A. M. Liberman, F. S. Cooper, D. P. Shankweiler, and M. Studdert-Kennedy, “Perception of the speech code,” Psychol. Rev. 74, 431–461 (1967). [CrossRef]
  3. T. Regier and P. Kay, “Language, thought, and color: Whorf was half right,” Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 439–446 (2009). [CrossRef]
  4. A. L. Gilbert, T. Regier, and P. Kay, “ Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not the left,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 103, 489–494 (2006). [CrossRef]
  5. P. Athanasopoulos, “Cognitive representation of colour in bilinguals: the case of Greek blues,” Bilingualism Language Cognition 12, 83–95 (2009). [CrossRef]
  6. P. Athanasopoulos, A. Wiggett, B. Dering, J.-R. Kuipers, and G. Thierry, “The Whorfian mind: electrophysiological evidence that language shapes perception,” Commun. Integr. Biol. 2, 332–334 (2009). [CrossRef]
  7. P. Athanasopoulos, L. Damjanovic, A. Krajciova, and M. Sasaki, “Representation of colour concepts in bilingual cognition: the case of Japanese blues,” Bilingualism Language Cognition 14, 9–17 (2011). [CrossRef]
  8. I. S. P. Nation, Teaching and Learning Vocabulary (Newbury House, 1990).
  9. B. C. Regan, J. P. Reffin, and J. D. Mollon, “Luminance noise and the rapid determination of discrimination ellipses in colour deficiency,” Vis. Res. 34, 1279–1299 (1994). [CrossRef]
  10. S. Wuerger, K. Xiao, C. Fu, and D. Karatzas, “Colour-opponent mechanisms are not affected by age-related chromatic sensitivity changes,” Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 30, 653–659 (2010). [CrossRef]
  11. S. M. Wuerger, P. Atkinson, and S. Cropper, “The cone inputs to the unique-hue mechanisms,” Vis. Res. 45, 3210–3223 (2005). [CrossRef]
  12. C. Witzel and K. R. Gegenfurtner, “Is there a lateralized category effect for color?” J. Vision 11, 16 (2011). [CrossRef]
  13. A. M. Brown, D. T. Lindsey, and K. M. Guckes, “Color names, color categories, and color-cued visual search: sometimes, color perception is not categorical.” J. Vision 11, 2 (2011). [CrossRef]
  14. S. M. Wuerger, A. Ruppertsberg, S. Malek, M. Bertamini, and J. Martinovic, “The integration of local chromatic motion signals is sensitive to contrast polarity,” Vis. Neurosci. 28, 239–246(2011). [CrossRef]
  15. G. Paramei and J. Molyneux, “Colour identification speed as a test of the right visual field Whorfian effect,” Perception 40 (ECVP Abstract Suppl.), 81 (2011).
  16. M. J. Ruiz and J.-M. Hupé, “Stimulus saliency, not colour category boundary, accounts for ‘Whorfian’ effects in colour search tasks,” Perception 40, 196 (2011). [CrossRef]
  17. J. H. Hsiao and G. W. Cottrell, “What is the cause of left hemisphere lateralization of English visual word recognition? Pre-existing language lateralization, or task characteristics? in The Thirty-first Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (2009).
  18. Y. Guo and E. D. Burgund, “Task effects in the mid-fusiform gyrus: a comparison of orthographic, phonological, and semantic processing of Chinese characters,” Brain Lang. 115, 113–120 (2010). [CrossRef]
  19. L. H. Tan, H.-L. Liu, C. A. Perfetti, J. A. Spinks, P. T. Fox, and J.-H. Gao, “The neural system underlying Chinese logograph reading,” NeuroImage 13, 836–846 (2001). [CrossRef]

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.


« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited