OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

| OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION

  • Editor: Franco Gori
  • Vol. 29, Iss. 2 — Feb. 1, 2012
  • pp: A247–A257

Context-dependent judgments of color that might allow color constancy in scenes with multiple regions of illumination

R. J. Lee and H. E. Smithson  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA A, Vol. 29, Issue 2, pp. A247-A257 (2012)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.29.00A247


View Full Text Article

Enhanced HTML    Acrobat PDF (1042 KB) Open Access





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

For a color-constant observer, a change in the spectral composition of the illumination is accompanied by a corresponding change in the chromaticity associated with an achromatic percept. However, maintaining color constancy for different regions of illumination within a scene implies the maintenance of multiple perceptual references. We investigated the features of a scene that enable the maintenance of separate perceptual references for two displaced but overlapping chromaticity distributions. The time-averaged, retinotopically localized stimulus was the primary determinant of color appearance judgments. However, spatial separation of test samples additionally served as a symbolic cue that allowed observers to maintain two separate perceptual references.

© 2012 Optical Society of America

OCIS Codes
(330.1690) Vision, color, and visual optics : Color
(330.1720) Vision, color, and visual optics : Color vision
(330.5020) Vision, color, and visual optics : Perception psychology
(330.5510) Vision, color, and visual optics : Psychophysics
(330.7320) Vision, color, and visual optics : Vision adaptation

ToC Category:
Color in natural or complex scenes

History
Original Manuscript: September 2, 2011
Revised Manuscript: November 18, 2011
Manuscript Accepted: December 2, 2011
Published: January 27, 2012

Virtual Issues
Vol. 7, Iss. 4 Virtual Journal for Biomedical Optics

Citation
R. J. Lee and H. E. Smithson, "Context-dependent judgments of color that might allow color constancy in scenes with multiple regions of illumination," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 29, A247-A257 (2012)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josaa/abstract.cfm?URI=josaa-29-2-A247


Sort:  Author  |  Year  |  Journal  |  Reset  

References

  1. M. J. Morgan, “On the scaling of size judgements by orientational cues,” Vis. Res. 32, 1433–1445 (1992). [CrossRef]
  2. H. Helson, “Adaptation-level as frame of reference for prediction of psychophysical data,” Am. J. Psychol. 60, 1–29 (1947). [CrossRef]
  3. H. Helson, “Adaptation-level as a basis for a quantitative theory of frames of reference,” Psychol. Rev. 55, 297–313 (1948). [CrossRef]
  4. P. Lennie and M. D’Zmura, “Mechanisms of color vision,” Crit. Rev. Neurobiol. 3, 333–401 (1988).
  5. J. von Kries, “Beitrag zur Physiologie der Gesichtsempndungen [Transl. Physiology of visual sensations],” in Sources of Color Science, D. L. MacAdam, ed. (MIT Press, 1878), pp. 101–108.
  6. A. Stockman, M. Langendörfer, H. E. Smithson, and L. T. Sharpe, “Human cone light adaptation: from behavioral measurements to molecular mechanisms,” J. Vision 6(6), 1194–1213 (2006). [CrossRef]
  7. E. N. J. Pugh and J. D. Mollon, “A theory of the Pi1 and Pi3 color mechanisms of stiles,” Vis. Res. 19, 293–312 (1979). [CrossRef]
  8. J. Krauskopf, D. R. Williams, and D. W. Heeley, “Cardinal directions of color space,” Vis. Res. 22, 1123–1131 (1982). [CrossRef]
  9. C. Tailby, S. G. Solomon, N. T. Dhruv, and P. Lennie, “Habituation reveals fundamental chromatic mechanisms in striate cortex of macaque,” J. Neurosci. 28, 1131–1139 (2008). [CrossRef]
  10. A. Kohn, “Visual adaptation: physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits,” J. Neurophysiol. 97, 3155–3164 (2007). [CrossRef]
  11. I. J. Murray, A. Daugirdiene, R. Stanikunas, H. Vaitkevicius, and J. J. Kulikowski, “Cone contrasts do not predict color constancy,” Vis. Neurosci. 23, 543–547 (2006).
  12. M. A. Webster and J. A. Wilson, “Interactions between chromatic adaptation and contrast adaptation in color appearance,” Vis. Res. 40, 3801–3816 (2000). [CrossRef]
  13. D. Katz, The World of Colour (K. Paul, Trench, Trubner, 1935).
  14. B. H. Crawford, “Visual adaptation in relation to brief conditioning stimuli,” Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B 134, 283–302(1947). [CrossRef]
  15. D. Jameson, L. M. Hurvich, and F. D. Varner, “Receptoral and postreceptoral visual processes in recovery from chromatic adaptation,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 76, 3034–3038 (1979). [CrossRef]
  16. J. Neitz, J. Carroll, Y. Yamauchi, M. Neitz, and D. R. Williams, “Color perception is mediated by a plastic neural mechanism that is adjustable in adults,” Neuron 35, 783–792 (2002). [CrossRef]
  17. P. B. Delahunt, M. A. Webster, L. Ma, and J. S. Werner, “Long-term renormalization of chromatic mechanisms following cataract surgery,” Vis. Neurosci. 21, 301–307 (2004). [CrossRef]
  18. D. H. Foster and S. M. C. Nascimento, “Relational colour constancy from invariant cone-excitation ratios,” Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B 257, 115–121 (1994). [CrossRef]
  19. H. E. Smithson, “Sensory, computational and cognitive components of human colour constancy,” Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B 360, 1329–1346 (2005). [CrossRef]
  20. D. I. A. MacLeod and S. He, “Visible flicker from invisible patterns,” Nature 361, 256–258 (1993). [CrossRef]
  21. S. He and D. I. A. MacLeod, “Local nonlinearity in S-cones and their estimated light-collecting apertures,” Vis. Res. 38, 1001–1006 (1998). [CrossRef]
  22. D. I. A. MacLeod, D. R. Williams, and W. Makous, “A visual nonlinearity fed by single cones,” Vis. Res. 32, 347–363 (1992). [CrossRef]
  23. S. J. Schein and R. Desimone, “Spectral properties of V4 neurons in the macaque,” J. Neurosci. 10, 3369–3389 (1990).
  24. A. C. Hurlbert and T. A. Poggio, “Synthesizing a color algorithm from examples,” Science 239, 482–485 (1988). [CrossRef]
  25. J. D. Mollon, B. C. Regan, and J. K. Bowmaker, “What is the function of the cone-rich rim of the retina?” Eye 12, 548–552 (1998). [CrossRef]
  26. M. D’Zmura and P. Lennie, “Mechanisms of color constancy,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 3, 1662–72 (1986). [CrossRef]
  27. R. J. Lee, K. A. Dawson, and H. E. Smithson, “Slow updating of the achromatic point after a change in illumination,” J. Vision12(1), 1–22 (2012). [CrossRef]
  28. H. E. Smithson and Q. Zaidi, “Colour constancy in context: Roles for local adaptation and levels of reference,” J. Vision 4(8), 693–710 (2004). [CrossRef]
  29. A. D. D’Antona and S. K. Shevell, “Induced steady color shifts from temporally varying surrounds,” Vis. Neurosci. 23, 483–487(2006). [CrossRef]
  30. K. Koffka, Principles of Gestalt Psychology (Harcourt, Brace, and World, 1935).
  31. A. Gelb, “Die Farbenkonstanz der Sehdinge,” in Handbuch der normalen und pathologischen Psychologie, A. Bethe, G. V. Bergmann, G. Embden, and A. Ellinger, eds. (Springer-Verlag, 1929), pp. 594–678.
  32. M. J. Morgan, S. N. Watamaniuk, and S. P. McKee, “The use of an implicit standard for measuring discrimination thresholds,” Vis. Res. 40, 2341–2349 (2000). [CrossRef]
  33. S. X. Xian, and S. K. Shevell, “Changes in color appearance caused by perceptual grouping,” Vis. Neurosci. 21, 383–388 (2004). [CrossRef]
  34. D. I. A. MacLeod and R. M. Boynton, “Chromaticity diagram showing cone excitation by stimuli of equal luminance,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 69, 1183–1186 (1979). [CrossRef]
  35. A. Stockman and L. T. Sharpe, “The spectral sensitivities of the middle- and long-wavelength-sensitive cones derived from measurements in observers of known genotype,” Vis. Res. 40, 1711–1737 (2000). [CrossRef]
  36. J. Golz and D. I. A. MacLeod, “Influence of scene statistics on colour constancy,” Nature 415, 637–640 (2002). [CrossRef]
  37. F. A. Wichmann and N. J. Hill, “The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit,” Percept. Psychophys. 63, 1293–1313 (2001). [CrossRef]
  38. D. H. Foster, “Color constancy,” Vis. Res. 51, 674–700 (2011). [CrossRef]
  39. Q. Zaidi, B. Spehar, and J. DeBonet, “Color constancy in variegated scenes: role of low-level mechanisms in discounting illumination changes,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 14, 2608–2621(1997). [CrossRef]
  40. P. Monnier and S. K. Shevell, “Large shifts in color appearance from patterned chromatic backgrounds,” Nat. Neurosci. 6, 801–802 (2003). [CrossRef]
  41. D. H. Foster, S. M. C. Nascimento, K. Amano, L. Arend, K. J. Linnell, J. L. Nieves, S. Plet, and J. S. Foster, “Parallel detection of violations of color constancy,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98, 8151–8156 (2001). [CrossRef]
  42. D. H. Foster, “Does colour constancy exist?,” Trends Cognit. Sci. 7, 439–443 (2003). [CrossRef]
  43. J. L. Barbur and K. Spang, “Colour constancy and conscious perception of changes of illuminant,” Neuropsychologia 46, 853–863 (2008). [CrossRef]
  44. R. O. Brown, “The world is not gray,” Investig. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 35, 2165 (1994).
  45. F. W. Cornelissen and E. Brenner, “Simultaneous colour constancy revisited: an analysis of viewing strategies,” Vis. Res. 35, 2431–48 (1995).
  46. E. Brenner, J. J. M. Granzier, and J. B. J. Smeets, “Perceiving colour at a glimpse: the relevance of where one fixates,” Vis. Res. 47, 2557–2568 (2007). [CrossRef]

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.


« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited