OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

| OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION

  • Editor: Franco Gori
  • Vol. 30, Iss. 3 — Mar. 1, 2013
  • pp: 342–352

Transparency perception: the key to understanding simultaneous color contrast

Vebjørn Ekroll and Franz Faul  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA A, Vol. 30, Issue 3, pp. 342-352 (2013)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.30.000342


View Full Text Article

Enhanced HTML    Acrobat PDF (686 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

The well-known simultaneous color contrast effect is traditionally explained in terms of visual color constancy mechanisms correcting for the confounding influence of ambient illumination on the retinal color signal. Recent research, however, suggests that the traditional gross quantitative laws of simultaneous color contrast, which are readily compatible with this functional explanation, should be revised and replaced by others, which are not readily understandable in terms of this perspective. Here, we show that the revised laws of simultaneous color contrast are well accounted for by an alternative theory explaining the simultaneous contrast effect in terms of mechanisms subserving the perception of transparent media.

© 2013 Optical Society of America

OCIS Codes
(330.0330) Vision, color, and visual optics : Vision, color, and visual optics
(330.1710) Vision, color, and visual optics : Color, measurement
(330.1720) Vision, color, and visual optics : Color vision

ToC Category:
Vision, Color, and Visual Optics

History
Original Manuscript: June 21, 2012
Revised Manuscript: November 5, 2012
Manuscript Accepted: December 31, 2012
Published: February 8, 2013

Virtual Issues
Vol. 8, Iss. 4 Virtual Journal for Biomedical Optics

Citation
Vebjørn Ekroll and Franz Faul, "Transparency perception: the key to understanding simultaneous color contrast," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 30, 342-352 (2013)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josaa/abstract.cfm?URI=josaa-30-3-342


Sort:  Author  |  Year  |  Journal  |  Reset  

References

  1. F. Metelli, “The perception of transparency,” Sci. Am. 230, 90–98 (1974). [CrossRef]
  2. M. A. Webster, “Light adaptation, contrast adaptation, and human color vision,” in Colour—Mind and the Physical World, R. Mausfeld and D. Heyer, eds. (Oxford University, 2003).
  3. M. A. Webster and J. A. Wilson, “Interactions between chromatic adaptation and contrast adaptation in color appearance,” Vis. Res. 40, 3801–3816 (2000). [CrossRef]
  4. M. Webster and D. Leonard, “Adaptation and perceptual norms in color vision,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 25, 2817–2825 (2008). [CrossRef]
  5. R. O. Brown and D. I. A. MacLeod, “Color appearance depends on the variance of surround colors,” Curr. Biol. 7, 844–849 (1997). [CrossRef]
  6. F. Faul, V. Ekroll, and G. Wendt, “Color appearance: the limited role of chromatic surround variance in the ‘gamut expansion effect,’” J. Vis. 8(3):30, 1–20 (2008). [CrossRef]
  7. V. Ekroll, F. Faul, and G. Wendt, “The strengths of simultaneous colour contrast and the gamut expansion effect correlate across observers: evidence for a common mechanism,” Vis. Res. 51, 311–322 (2011).
  8. E. Brenner and F. Cornelissen, “The influence of chromatic and achromatic variability on chromatic induction and perceived colour,” Perception 31, 225–232 (2002). [CrossRef]
  9. E. Brenner, J. Ruiz, E. Herráiz, F. Cornelissen, and J. Smeets, “Chromatic induction and the layout of colours within a complex scene,” Vis. Res. 43, 1413–1421 (2003). [CrossRef]
  10. V. Ekroll and F. Faul, “New laws of simultaneous colour contrast?” Seeing Perceiving 25, 107–141 (2012). [CrossRef]
  11. V. Ekroll and F. Faul, “Basic characteristics of simultaneous colour contrast revisited,” Psychol. Sci. 23, 1246–1255 (2012). [CrossRef]
  12. O. Rinner and K. R. Gegenfurtner, “Time course of chromatic adaptation for color appearance and discrimination,” Vis. Res. 40, 1813–1826 (2000). [CrossRef]
  13. J. Walraven, “Discounting the background—The missing link in the explanation of chromatic induction,” Vis. Res. 16, 289–295 (1976). [CrossRef]
  14. P. Whittle and P. D. C. Challands, “The effect of background luminance on the brightness of flashes,” Vis. Res. 9, 1095–1110(1969). [CrossRef]
  15. H. von Helmholtz, ed., Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik (Verlag von Leopold Vos, 1911).
  16. J. Walraven, T. L. Benzschawel, and B. E. Rogowitz, “Color-constancy interpretation of chromatic induction,” Die Farbe 34, 269–273 (1987).
  17. R. B. Lotto and D. Purves, “From the cover: an empirical explanation of color contrast,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 12834–12839 (2000). [CrossRef]
  18. E. Hering, Grundzüge der Lehre vom Lichtsinn (Verlag von Julius Springer, 1920).
  19. F. Metelli, “An algebraic development of the theory of perceptual transparency,” Ergonomics 13, 59–66 (1970). [CrossRef]
  20. A. Kirschmann, “Some effects of contrast,” Am. J. Psychol. 4, 542–557 (1892). [CrossRef]
  21. J. Bosten and J. Mollon, “Kirschmann’s fourth law,” Vis. Res. 53, 40–46 (2012). [CrossRef]
  22. V. Ekroll, F. Faul, and R. Niederée, “The peculiar nature of simultaneous colour contrast in uniform surrounds,” Vis. Res 44, 1765–1786 (2004). [CrossRef]
  23. V. Ekroll and F. Faul, “A simple model describes large individual differences in simultaneous colour contrast,” Vis. Res. 49, 2261–2272 (2009). [CrossRef]
  24. D. H. Brainard, “Calibration of a computer controlled color monitor,” Color Res. Appl. 14, 23–34 (1989). [CrossRef]
  25. J. Golz and D. I. A. MacLeod, “Colorimetry for CRT displays,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 20, 769–781 (2003). [CrossRef]
  26. A. Stockman, D. I. MacLeod, and N. E. Johnson, “Spectral sensitivities of the human cones,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 10, 2491–2521 (1993). [CrossRef]
  27. J. Foley, A. Van Dam, S. Feiner, and J. Hughes, Computer Graphics: Principles and Practice in C (Addison-Wesley, 1995).
  28. V. J. Chen and M. D’Zmura, “Test of a convergence model for color transparency perception,” Perception 27, 595–608 (1998). [CrossRef]
  29. M. D’Zmura, P. Colantoni, K. Knoblauch, and B. Laget, “Color transparency,” Perception 26, 471–492 (1997). [CrossRef]
  30. F. Faul, “Theoretische und experimentelle Untersuchung chromatischer Determinanten perzeptueller Transparenz,” Dissertation (Christian-Albrechts-Universität, 1997).
  31. R. Kasrai and F. A. A. Kingdom, “Precision, accuracy, and range of perceived achromatic transparency,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 18, 1–11 (2001). [CrossRef]
  32. M. Singh and B. L. Anderson, “Toward a perceptual theory of transparency,” Psychol. Rev. 109, 492–519 (2002).
  33. H. Helson and W. C. Michels, “The effect of chromatic adaptation on achromaticity,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 38, 1025–1032 (1948). [CrossRef]
  34. D. Brainard and B. Wandell, “Asymmetric color matching: how color appearance depends on the illuminant,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 9, 1433–1448 (1992). [CrossRef]
  35. D. I. A. MacLeod and R. M. Boynton, “Chromaticity diagram showing cone excitation by stimuli of equal luminance,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 69, 1183–1186 (1979). [CrossRef]
  36. B. Khang and Q. Zaidi, “Accuracy of color scission for spectral transparencies,” J. Vis. 2(6):3, 451–466 (2002). [CrossRef]
  37. B. G. Khang and Q. Zaidi, “Cues and strategies for color constancy: perceptual scission, image junctions and transformational color matching,” Vis. Res. 42, 211–226. (2002). [CrossRef]
  38. A. Gilchrist, C. Kossyfidis, F. Bonato, T. Agostini, J. Cataliotti, X. Li, B. Spehar, V. Annan, and E. Economou, “An anchoring theory of lightness perception,” Psychol. Rev. 106, 795–834 (1999). [CrossRef]
  39. R. Mausfeld, “Color perception: from Grassman codes to a dual code for object and illumination colors,” in Color Vision, W. G. K. Backhaus, R. Kliegl, and J. S. Werner, eds. (De Gruyter, 1998), pp. 219–250.
  40. E. Brenner and F. W. Cornelissen, “Spatial interactions in color vision depend on distances between boundaries,” Naturwissenschaften 78, 70–73 (1991). [CrossRef]
  41. V. Ekroll, F. Faul, R. Niederée, and E. Richter, “The natural center of chromaticity space is not always achromatic: a new look at color induction,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 99, 13352–13356 (2002). [CrossRef]
  42. R. M. Evans, “Variables of perceived color,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 54, 1467–1474 (1964). [CrossRef]
  43. P. Heggelund, “Simultaneous luminance contrast with chromatic colors,” Vis. Res. 33, 1709–1722 (1993). [CrossRef]
  44. R. Niederée, “More than three dimensions: what continuity considerations can tell us about perceived colour,” in Color Ontology and Color Science, J. Cohen and M. Matthen, eds. (MIT, 2010), pp. 91–122.
  45. N. Umbach and J. Heller, “Dimensionality of achromatic color space,” Perception40, ECVP Abstract Supplement (2011), p. 196.
  46. P. Whittle, “Contrast colours,” in Colour Perception: Mind and the Physical World, R. Mausfeld and D. Heyer, eds. (Oxford University, 2003), pp. 115–138.
  47. W. Richards, J. Koenderink, and A. van Doorn, “Transparency and imaginary colors,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 26, 1119–1128 (2009). [CrossRef]
  48. R. B. Lotto and D. Purves, “The empirical basis of color perception,” Conscious Cogn. 11, 609–629 (2002). [CrossRef]
  49. D. H. Brainard, “Color constancy in the nearly natural image. 2. achromatic loci,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 15, 307–325 (1998). [CrossRef]
  50. R. M. Evans, The Perception of Color (Wiley, 1974).
  51. P. Whittle, “The psychophysics of contrast brightness,” in Lightness, Brightness and Transparency, A. L. Gilchrist, ed. (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1994), pp. 35–110.
  52. P. Whittle, “Contrast brightness and ordinary seeing,” in Lightness, Brightness and Transparency, A. L. Gilchrist, ed. (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1994), pp. 111–158.
  53. H. Meyer, “Über Kontrast- und Komplementärfarben,” Ann. Phys. XCV, 170–171 (1855). [CrossRef]
  54. E. Hering, “Ueber die Theorie des simultanen Contrastes von Helmholtz: II. Der Contrastversuch von H. Meyer und die Versuche am Farbenkreisel,” Pflügers Archiv für Physiologie 41, 1–29 (1887). [CrossRef]
  55. S. Runeson, “On the possibility of “smart” perceptual mechanisms,” Scand. J. Psychol. 18, 172–179 (1977). [CrossRef]
  56. R. Mausfeld, “The physicalistic trap in perception theory,” in Perception and the Physical World: Psychological and Philosophical Issues in Perception, D. Heyer and R. Mausfeld, eds. (Wiley, 2002), pp. 75–112.
  57. R. Mausfeld, “‘Colour’ as part of the format of different perceptual primitives: the dual coding of colour,” in Colour Perception: Mind and the Physical World, R. Mausfeld and D. Heyer, eds. (Oxford University, 2003), pp. 381–429.
  58. R. Mausfeld, “Colour within an internalist framework: the role of ‘colour’ in the structure of the perceptual system,” in Color Ontology and Color Science, J. Cohen and M. Matthen, eds. (MIT, 2010), pp. 381–429.
  59. B. L. Anderson, “A theory of illusory lightness and transparency in monocular and binocular images: the role of contour junctions,” Perception 26, 419–453 (1997). [CrossRef]
  60. B. L. Anderson and J. Winawer, “Image segmentation and lightness perception,” Nature 434, 79–83 (2005). [CrossRef]
  61. D. Wollschläger and B. L. Anderson, “The role of layered scene representations in color appearance,” Curr. Biol. 19, 430–435 (2009). [CrossRef]
  62. F. A. A. Kingdom, “Lightness, brightness and transparency: a quarter century of new ideas, captivating demonstrations and unrelenting controversy,” Vis. Res. 51, 652–673 (2011). [CrossRef]

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.


« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited