OSA's Digital Library

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

Journal of the Optical Society of America A

| OPTICS, IMAGE SCIENCE, AND VISION

  • Editor: Franco Gori
  • Vol. 31, Iss. 4 — Apr. 1, 2014
  • pp: A179–A185

Color constancy influenced by unnatural spatial structure

Yoko Mizokami and Hirohisa Yaguchi  »View Author Affiliations


JOSA A, Vol. 31, Issue 4, pp. A179-A185 (2014)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/JOSAA.31.00A179


View Full Text Article

Enhanced HTML    Acrobat PDF (685 KB)





Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Browse by Journal and Year


   


Lookup Conference Papers

Close Browse Journals / Lookup Meetings

Article Tools

Share
Citations

Abstract

The recognition of spatial structures is important for color constancy because we cannot identify an object’s color under different illuminations without knowing which space it is in and how that space is illuminated. To show the importance of the natural structure of environments on color constancy, we investigated the way in which color appearance was affected by unnatural viewing conditions in which a spatial structure was distorted. Observers judged the color of a test patch placed in the center of a small room illuminated by white or reddish lights, as well as two rooms illuminated by white and reddish light, respectively. In the natural viewing condition, an observer saw the room(s) through a viewing window, whereas in an unnatural viewing condition, the scene structure was scrambled by a kaleidoscope-type viewing box. Results of single room condition with one illuminant color showed little difference in color constancy between the two viewing conditions. However, it decreased in the two-rooms condition with a more complex arrangement of space and illumination. The patch’s appearance under the unnatural viewing condition was more influenced by simultaneous contrast than its appearance under the natural viewing condition. It also appears that color appearance under white illumination is more stable compared to that under reddish illumination. These findings suggest that natural spatial structure plays an important role for color constancy in a complex environment.

© 2014 Optical Society of America

OCIS Codes
(330.0330) Vision, color, and visual optics : Vision, color, and visual optics
(330.1720) Vision, color, and visual optics : Color vision
(330.5510) Vision, color, and visual optics : Psychophysics

ToC Category:
Color and lightness constancy

History
Original Manuscript: October 3, 2013
Revised Manuscript: December 7, 2013
Manuscript Accepted: December 8, 2013
Published: February 6, 2014

Virtual Issues
Vol. 9, Iss. 6 Virtual Journal for Biomedical Optics

Citation
Yoko Mizokami and Hirohisa Yaguchi, "Color constancy influenced by unnatural spatial structure," J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 31, A179-A185 (2014)
http://www.opticsinfobase.org/josaa/abstract.cfm?URI=josaa-31-4-A179


Sort:  Author  |  Year  |  Journal  |  Reset  

References

  1. D. H. Foster, “Color constancy,” Vis. Res. 51, 674–700 (2011). [CrossRef]
  2. J. von Kries, “Chromatic adaptation,” in Sources of Color Vision, D. L. MacAdam, ed. (MIT, 1970), pp. 109–119.
  3. G. Buchsbaum, “A spatial processor model for object colour perception,” J. Franklin Inst. 310, 1–26 (1980). [CrossRef]
  4. H. Helson, “Some factors and implications of color constancy,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 33, 555–567 (1943). [CrossRef]
  5. M. D’Zmura and P. Lennie, “Mechanisms of color constancy,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 3, 1662–1672 (1986). [CrossRef]
  6. M. G. Bloj, D. Kersten, and A. C. Hurlbert, “Perception of three-dimensional shape influences colour perception through mutual illumination,” Nature 402, 877–879 (1999).
  7. Q. Zaidi, B. Spehar, and J. DeBonet, “Color constancy in variegated scenes: role of low-level mechanisms in discounting illumination changes,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 14, 2608–2621 (1997). [CrossRef]
  8. K. Amano and D. H. Foster, “Colour constancy under simultaneous changes in surface position and illuminant,” Proc. Biol. Sci. 271, 2319–2326 (2004).
  9. J. Golz and D. I. MacLeod, “Influence of scene statistics on colour constancy,” Nature 415, 637–640 (2002). [CrossRef]
  10. F. A. Kingdom, “Color brings relief to human vision,” Nat. Neurosci. 6, 641–644 (2003). [CrossRef]
  11. E. H. Land and J. J. McCann, “Lightness and retinex theory,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. 61, 1–11 (1971). [CrossRef]
  12. D. H. Brainard and W. T. Freeman, “Bayesian color constancy,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 14, 1393–1411 (1997). [CrossRef]
  13. L. T. Maloney, K. R. Gegenfurtner, and L. T. Sharpe, “Physics-based approaches to modeling surface color perception. Color vision: from genes to perception,” in Color Vision: From Genes to Perception (Cambridge University, 1999), pp. 387–422.
  14. D. H. Brainard, W. A. Brunt, and J. M. Speigle, “Color constancy in the nearly natural image. I. Asymmetric matches,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 14, 2091–2110 (1997). [CrossRef]
  15. D. H. Brainard, “Color constancy in the nearly natural image. 2. Achromatic loci,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 15, 307–325 (1998). [CrossRef]
  16. L. Arend and A. Reeves, “Simultaneous color constancy,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 3, 1743–1751 (1986). [CrossRef]
  17. M. Hedrich, M. Bloj, and A. I. Ruppertsberg, “Color constancy improves for real 3D objects,” J. Vis. 9(16), 11–16 (2009).
  18. M. Ikeda, Y. Hattori, and H. Shinoda, “Color modification of pictures requiring same color impression as real scene,” Opt. Rev. 9, 282–292 (2002). [CrossRef]
  19. Y. Mizokami, M. Ikeda, and H. Shinoda, “Color constancy in a photograph perceived as a three-dimensional space,” Opt. Rev. 11, 288–296 (2004). [CrossRef]
  20. Y. Mizokami, M. Ikeda, and H. Shinoda, “Lightness change as perceived in relation to the size of recognized visual space of illumination,” Opt. Rev. 5, 315–319 (1998). [CrossRef]
  21. Y. Mizokami, M. Ikeda, and H. Shinoda, “Color property of the recognized visual space of illumination controlled by interior color as the initial visual information,” Opt. Rev. 7, 358–363 (2000). [CrossRef]
  22. M. Ikeda, Y. Mizokami, and H. Shinoda, “Three dimensionality of the recognized visual space of illumination proved by hidden illumination,” Opt. Rev. 5, 200–205 (1998). [CrossRef]
  23. L. E. Arend, A. Reeves, J. Schirillo, and R. Goldstein, “Simultaneous color constancy: paper with diverse Munsell values,” J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 8, 661–672 (1991). [CrossRef]
  24. J. N. Yang and S. K. Shevell, “Stereo disparity improves color constancy,” Vis. Res. 42, 1979–1989 (2002). [CrossRef]
  25. P. B. Delahunt and D. H. Brainard, “Does human color constancy incorporate the statistical regularity of natural daylight?” J. Vis. 4(2):1, 57–81 (2004). [CrossRef]
  26. J. M. Kraft, S. I. Maloney, and D. H. Brainard, “Surface-illuminant ambiguity and color constancy: effects of scene complexity and depth cues,” Perception 31, 247–263 (2002). [CrossRef]
  27. A. L. Gilchrist, “Perceived lightness depends on perceived spatial arrangement,” Science 195, 185–187 (1977). [CrossRef]
  28. K. Rattanakasamsuk and H. Shinoda, “Partial exclusion of the local effect from the assessment of recognized illuminant using depth separation,” Opt. Rev. 13, 380–387 (2006). [CrossRef]
  29. J. N. Yang and S. K. Shevell, “Surface color perception under two illuminants: the second illuminant reduces color constancy,” J. Vis. 3(5):4, 369–379 (2003). [CrossRef]
  30. I. Kuriki, S. Nakadomari, and K. Kitahara, “A case of extreme simultaneous color-contrast in a patient with hypoxic encephalopathy,” Vision 15(4), 223–244 (2003).
  31. M. D’Zmura, K. Knoblauch, M. A. Henaff, and F. Michel, “Dependence of color on context in a case of cortical color vision deficiency,” Vis. Res. 38, 3455–3459 (1998). [CrossRef]
  32. A. Gilchrist, C. Kossyfidis, F. Bonato, T. Agostini, J. Cataliotti, X. Li, B. Spehar, V. Annan, and E. Economou, “An anchoring theory of lightness perception,” Psycholog. Rev. 106, 795–834 (1999). [CrossRef]
  33. A. L. Gilchrist and A. Radonjic, “Functional frameworks of illumination revealed by probe disk technique,” J. Vis. 10(5):6 (2010). [CrossRef]

Cited By

Alert me when this paper is cited

OSA is able to provide readers links to articles that cite this paper by participating in CrossRef's Cited-By Linking service. CrossRef includes content from more than 3000 publishers and societies. In addition to listing OSA journal articles that cite this paper, citing articles from other participating publishers will also be listed.


« Previous Article  |  Next Article »

OSA is a member of CrossRef.

CrossCheck Deposited